The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making 2015
DOI: 10.1002/9781118468333.ch6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Overprecision in Judgment

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
61
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 97 publications
(66 citation statements)
references
References 157 publications
2
61
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Still, we cannot rule out the possibility that high precision and high confidence go together in statements experts actually make. The conversational norms‐account of overprecision (Moore et al, ) suggests that people try to make their statements as informative as possible. This implies that an expert might try to simultaneously maximize both precision and confidence, at the risk of exaggerating both.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Still, we cannot rule out the possibility that high precision and high confidence go together in statements experts actually make. The conversational norms‐account of overprecision (Moore et al, ) suggests that people try to make their statements as informative as possible. This implies that an expert might try to simultaneously maximize both precision and confidence, at the risk of exaggerating both.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When different groups are instructed to estimate 90 and 50% intervals, they do not appear to be sensitive to the assigned confidence level and produce intervals of the same width (Langnickel & Zeisberger, ; Teigen & Jørgensen, ), leading to overprecise 90% intervals but more well‐calibrated 50% intervals. Thus, overprecision can in part be attributed to a deficient understanding of probability distributions (Moore, Tenney, & Haran, ).…”
Section: Overprecision and The Preciseness Paradoxmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A better approach to the study of overprecision asks people to specify a confidence interval around their estimates, such as a confidence interval that is wide enough that there is a 90% chance the right answer is inside it and only a 10% chance the right answer is outside it (Alpert & Raiffa, 1982). Results routinely find that hit rates inside 90% confidence intervals are below 50%, implying that people set their ranges too precisely-acting as if they are inappropriately confident their beliefs are accurate (Moore, Tenney, & Haran, 2016). This effect even holds across levels of expertise (Atir, Rosenzweig, & Dunning, 2015;McKenzie, Liersch, & Yaniv, 2008).…”
Section: Overprecisionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In evaluating the performance of probability forecasters, we assess their accuracy and their confidence. While alternative definitions of accuracy exist in the literature on evaluating subjective probabilities (e.g., Lichtenstein, Fischhoff, & Phillips, ; Moore, Tenney, & Haran, ; Yaniv & Foster, ), we will hereafter consider accuracy to be a measure of the degree of correspondence between the participants' predictions (which are the probabilities of event occurrences) and the observed outcomes. The average Brier score (Brier, ) is used to evaluate and compare participants' long‐term accuracy.…”
Section: Performance Measures and The Value Of Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%