1984
DOI: 10.5951/jresematheduc.15.5.0323
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Order and Equivalence of Rational Numbers: A Clinical Teaching Experiment

Abstract: Fourth-grade students' understanding of the order and equivalence of rational numbers was investigated in 11 interviews with each of 12 children during an 18-week teaching experiment. Six children were instructed individually and as a group at each of two sites. The instruction relied heavily on the use of manipulative aids. Children's explanations of their responses to interview tasks were used to identify strategies for comparing fraction pairs of three types: same numerators, same denominators, and differen… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
21
0
4

Year Published

1999
1999
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 30 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
1
21
0
4
Order By: Relevance
“…A similar phenomenon occurs when comparing fractions without common components. Students tend to think that a fraction is larger if the numerator and denominator are larger (Behr et al 1984;Gelman et al 1989;Resnick et al 1989). Accordingly, they perform better on items where the largest fraction has the largest numerator and denominator (so-called congruent items, for example: 2/3 vs. 7/9) than on items where the largest fraction has the smallest numerator and denominator (incongruent items, for example: 3/4 vs. 5/9).…”
Section: Natural Number Bias In Numerical Rational Number Size Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A similar phenomenon occurs when comparing fractions without common components. Students tend to think that a fraction is larger if the numerator and denominator are larger (Behr et al 1984;Gelman et al 1989;Resnick et al 1989). Accordingly, they perform better on items where the largest fraction has the largest numerator and denominator (so-called congruent items, for example: 2/3 vs. 7/9) than on items where the largest fraction has the smallest numerator and denominator (incongruent items, for example: 3/4 vs. 5/9).…”
Section: Natural Number Bias In Numerical Rational Number Size Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Enright (1990), Behr, Wachsmuth, Post, and Lesh (1984), and Steffe and Olive (1991) proposed common errors and misconceptions in fraction-related items; but these studies were limited in which they did not speculate about error patterns across items. In addition, Brown and Quinn (2006) classified 25 fraction problems into six categories, analyzed errors, and provided the descriptions of common errors students made in each item category.…”
Section: Framework For This Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The other is the very large literature on students' knowledge and learning of school mathematics concepts and procedures, particularly number concepts and operations. This work has produced detailed analyses of whole number concepts (Fuson, 1992;Resnick, 1983), additive relationships (Carpenter & Moser, 1984;Siegler & Jenkins, 1989), multiplicative relationships (Greer, 1992;Harel & Confrey, 1994), rational numbers (Behr, Wachsmuth, Post, & Lesh, 1984;Smith, 1995), and algebraic concepts and procedures (Kieran, 1992;Usiskin, 1988). Notably, far fewer studies have examined the development of students' spatial and geometric reasoning (Clements & Battista, 1992).…”
Section: Finding the Mathematics In Work Activitiesmentioning
confidence: 99%