2019
DOI: 10.1523/eneuro.0411-19.2019
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Optimized but Not Maximized Cue Integration for 3D Visual Perception

Abstract: Reconstructing three-dimensional (3D) scenes from two-dimensional (2D) retinal images is an ill-posed problem. Despite this, 3D perception of the world based on 2D retinal images is seemingly accurate and precise. The integration of distinct visual cues is essential for robust 3D perception in humans, but it is unclear whether this is true for non-human primates (NHPs). Here, we assessed 3D perception in macaque monkeys using a planar surface orientation discrimination task. Perception was accurate across a wi… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 65 publications
(125 reference statements)
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In previous work, we interpreted improvements in accuracy of 3D motion-in-depth judgments with feedback as the result of the inexperienced participants learning to overcome “flatness” priors developed through extensive real-world use of 2D displays and to compel recruitment of the available depth cues that they were otherwise discounting ( Fulvio & Rokers, 2017 ; Fulvio et al, 2020 ). Given that integration of multiple cues enhances reliability of visual perception ( Chang, Thompson, Doudlah, Kim, Sunkara, & Rosenberg, 2020 ; Hillis, Watt, Landy, & Banks, 2004 ; Knill & Saunders, 2003 ; Murphy, Ban, & Welchman, 2013 ; Oruç, Maloney, & Landy, 2013 ; Preston, Kourtzi, & Welchman, 2009 ; Rideaux & Welchman, 2018 ; Welchman, Deubelius, Conrad, BĂĽlthoff, & Kourtzi, 2005 ), increasing the number of cues integrated in estimating the motion direction of the target may be one way in which feedback reduces serial dependence. (We note, however, that our design leaves open the possibility that other factors may also have accounted for the effects of feedback, given that adjustment responses are subject to some motor error and possible biases toward or away from certain directions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In previous work, we interpreted improvements in accuracy of 3D motion-in-depth judgments with feedback as the result of the inexperienced participants learning to overcome “flatness” priors developed through extensive real-world use of 2D displays and to compel recruitment of the available depth cues that they were otherwise discounting ( Fulvio & Rokers, 2017 ; Fulvio et al, 2020 ). Given that integration of multiple cues enhances reliability of visual perception ( Chang, Thompson, Doudlah, Kim, Sunkara, & Rosenberg, 2020 ; Hillis, Watt, Landy, & Banks, 2004 ; Knill & Saunders, 2003 ; Murphy, Ban, & Welchman, 2013 ; Oruç, Maloney, & Landy, 2013 ; Preston, Kourtzi, & Welchman, 2009 ; Rideaux & Welchman, 2018 ; Welchman, Deubelius, Conrad, BĂĽlthoff, & Kourtzi, 2005 ), increasing the number of cues integrated in estimating the motion direction of the target may be one way in which feedback reduces serial dependence. (We note, however, that our design leaves open the possibility that other factors may also have accounted for the effects of feedback, given that adjustment responses are subject to some motor error and possible biases toward or away from certain directions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In classical psychophysical experiments on surface orientation, it is typical for the projected size of a stimulus on the retina to be held constant regardless of the surface slant. In these circumstances, slant (and presumably tilt) discrimination thresholds decrease as slant increases [2][3][4]35]. For surfaces of a given size, however, the projected size of the stimulus decreases as slant increases.…”
Section: Two Psychophysical Experimentsmentioning
confidence: 99%