2005
DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1906-05.2005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Optimal Compensation for Changes in Task-Relevant Movement Variability

Abstract: Effective movement planning should take into account the consequences of possible errors in executing a planned movement. These errors can result from either sensory uncertainty or variability in movement planning and production. We examined the ability of humans to compensate for variability in sensory estimation and movement production under conditions in which variability is increased artificially by the experimenter. Subjects rapidly pointed at a target region that had an adjacent penalty region. Target an… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

22
158
1

Year Published

2008
2008
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 167 publications
(181 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
22
158
1
Order By: Relevance
“…These findings stand in contrast to multiple demonstrations of Bayes-optimality (Doya 2007) in perceptual decision-making (Gold and Shadlen 2002;Knill and Pouget 2004), motor control (Trommershäuser et al 2003(Trommershäuser et al , 2005, multimodal integration (Körding and Wolpert 2004), reasoning (Oaksford and Chater 1994), and even setting metalearning parameters for reinforcement learning (Behrens et al 2007;Yu 2007). However, whereas Bayesian inference may be computationally feasible (and indeed, simple) in scenarios that can be reduced to a several-alternative forced-choice decision (Gold and Shadlen 2002) or a choice between lotteries (Wu et al 2009), representation learning in natural environments places much heavier computational demands on the learning system.…”
Section: Resultscontrasting
confidence: 97%
“…These findings stand in contrast to multiple demonstrations of Bayes-optimality (Doya 2007) in perceptual decision-making (Gold and Shadlen 2002;Knill and Pouget 2004), motor control (Trommershäuser et al 2003(Trommershäuser et al , 2005, multimodal integration (Körding and Wolpert 2004), reasoning (Oaksford and Chater 1994), and even setting metalearning parameters for reinforcement learning (Behrens et al 2007;Yu 2007). However, whereas Bayesian inference may be computationally feasible (and indeed, simple) in scenarios that can be reduced to a several-alternative forced-choice decision (Gold and Shadlen 2002) or a choice between lotteries (Wu et al 2009), representation learning in natural environments places much heavier computational demands on the learning system.…”
Section: Resultscontrasting
confidence: 97%
“…The actual performance is obviously poorer than this, but the difference is not very large. This confirms that near-optimal performance can be achieved through adjustments that are primarily based on recent experience (Brenner and Smeets 2011a;Narain et al 2013;Trommershäuser et al 2005;van Beers 2009van Beers , 2012.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…People are able to implicitly estimate the magnitude of their own variable error and use it to modify their movements in the light of the experiment's reward context 66,67 .…”
Section: Skilled Athletes Are Likely To Have Trained Their Decision Cmentioning
confidence: 99%