2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2015.05.016
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Optimal and human eye movements to clustered low value cues to increase decision rewards during search

Abstract: Rewards have important influences on the motor planning of primates and the firing of neurons coding visual information and action. When eye movements to a target are differentially rewarded across locations, primates execute saccades towards the possible target location with the highest expected value, a product of sensory evidence and potentially earned reward (saccade to maximum expected value model, sMEV). Yet, in the natural world eye movements are not directly rewarded. Their role is to gather informatio… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
5

Citation Types

0
28
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 84 publications
0
28
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In the present conceptualization, gaze target is chosen to reduce uncertainty in order to make better decisions about walking direction, and in the Reinforcement Learning framework, walking direction is chosen to maximize future expected discounted reward that has been learned through experience. As pointed out by Eckstein, Schoonveld, Zhang, Mack, & Akbas (2015), and others (Gottlieb et al, 2012; Gottlieb, Hayhoe, Hikosaka, & Rangel, 2014) eye movements themselves are not directly rewarded in the context of behavior, unlike many of the neurophysiological experiments as well as some of the psychophysical experiments (e.g., Navalpakkam et al, 2010, Schutz et al, 2012; Stritzke et al, 2009). Eckstein et al (2015) argue that, at least in some cases, it is the decision informed by the eye movement that gets the reward, not the eye movement per se.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…In the present conceptualization, gaze target is chosen to reduce uncertainty in order to make better decisions about walking direction, and in the Reinforcement Learning framework, walking direction is chosen to maximize future expected discounted reward that has been learned through experience. As pointed out by Eckstein, Schoonveld, Zhang, Mack, & Akbas (2015), and others (Gottlieb et al, 2012; Gottlieb, Hayhoe, Hikosaka, & Rangel, 2014) eye movements themselves are not directly rewarded in the context of behavior, unlike many of the neurophysiological experiments as well as some of the psychophysical experiments (e.g., Navalpakkam et al, 2010, Schutz et al, 2012; Stritzke et al, 2009). Eckstein et al (2015) argue that, at least in some cases, it is the decision informed by the eye movement that gets the reward, not the eye movement per se.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…Although replicating the essential features of instrumental sampling requires relatively complex sequential paradigms (10,29), extensive analyses and control conditions ruled out confounds that may arise in such paradigms. We discuss the implication of the findings from the perspectives of the valuebased and priority-based interpretations of LIP function.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A second key question is whether EIG is computed dynamically based on the uncertainty of each forthcoming action or relies on long-term estimates of average validity. A dynamic "look ahead" mechanism affords high flexibility, but it may be computationally expensive and may not be used consistently in all behavioral contexts (10,29). An important question for future research is to what extent EIG computations are flexible and responsive to rapid changes in context, or rely on stored validity representations to produce routine-based sampling policies (13,41,42).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Face processing has its own dedicated system in the brain (Kanwisher et al, 1997). Face recognition is a highly practiced task for which humans develop fixation strategies that remain very consistent across time (Peterson and Eckstein, 2013b;Mehoudar et al, 2014) and show a consistent optimality and rigidity Eckstein, 2013b, 2014) that is rarely present in simpler artificial laboratory tasks (Verghese, 2012;Ackermann and Landy, 2013;Eckstein et al, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Face recognition is a highly practiced task for which humans develop fixation strategies that remain very consistent across time (Peterson and Eckstein, 2013b;Mehoudar et al, 2014) and show a consistent optimality and rigidity Eckstein, 2013b, 2014) that is rarely present in simpler artificial laboratory tasks (Verghese, 2012;Ackermann and Landy, 2013;Eckstein et al, 2015). The frequently used fixation strategy with faces might give rise to special fixation-specific representations in face-related brain areas.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%