1998
DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00285-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Optic flow and the metric of the visual ground plane

Abstract: A theory is developed in which the optic flow of an observer translating over the ground plane determines the metric of egocentric visual space. Optic flow is used to operationalize the equality of spatial intervals not unlike physicists use time to compare spatial intervals. The theory predicts empirical matching ratios for collinear, sagittal intervals to within 2% of the mean (eight subjects, standard error also 2%). The theory predicts that frontoparallel intervals on the ground plane will match sagittal i… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

6
39
1

Year Published

2002
2002
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 41 publications
(46 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
6
39
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Another result obtained by Loomis and Philbeck (1999) is that the degree of perceptual distortion increased slightly with egocentric distance of the L-shaped objects, a result also apparent in the extent matching results of Beusmans (1998) and Loomis et al (1992). At first glance, it might appear that the increasing perceptual shape distortion with distance is the consequence of diminishing effectiveness of distance cues, such as accommodation, convergence, and binocular disparity.…”
mentioning
confidence: 87%
“…Another result obtained by Loomis and Philbeck (1999) is that the degree of perceptual distortion increased slightly with egocentric distance of the L-shaped objects, a result also apparent in the extent matching results of Beusmans (1998) and Loomis et al (1992). At first glance, it might appear that the increasing perceptual shape distortion with distance is the consequence of diminishing effectiveness of distance cues, such as accommodation, convergence, and binocular disparity.…”
mentioning
confidence: 87%
“…Optic flow cannot give information about absolute distance to an object and travel speed. Rather it can be used to compare spatial intervals [25] and for time measurements relative to the object and observer-the time-to-contact [6,26] (but see also [7]). Under certain conditions, optic flow has been shown to be a reliable cue to estimate distance of travel [27,28].…”
Section: Visual Cuesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although some of this work has focused on the metrics of visual space in general and has shown that visual space is curved (i.e., hyperbolic or elliptical; Battro, Netto, & Rozestraten, 1976;Blank, 1958;Koenderink, van Doorn, & Lappin, 2000;Luneburg, 1950), an abundance of other work has focused on how visual space is distorted in an affine manner, so that perceived intervals in depth (or parallel to the observer's line of sight) appear to become systematically compressed with depth, relative to perceived intervals in the horizontal or vertical directions (i.e., the frontoparallel plane perpendicular to the observer's line of sight; Beusmans, 1998;Foley, Ribeiro-Filho, & Da Silva, 2004;Gilinsky, 1951;Gogel, 1964;Haber, 1985;Harway, 1963;Hecht, van Doorn, & Koenderink, 1999;Kudoh, 2005;Loomis, Da Silva, Fujita, & Fukusima, 1992;Loomis & Philbeck, 1999;Matsushima, de Oliveira, Ribeiro-Filho, & Da Silva, 2005;Norman, Crabtree, Clayton, & Norman, 2005;Norman, Todd, Perotti, & Tittle, 1996;Todd & Norman, 2003;Todd, Tittle, & Norman, 1995;Toye, 1986;Wagner, 1985). Some of this work has shown that when extents are viewed in near space, depth intervals must be made approximately 1.5-2 times as large to be perceived to be the same as horizontal extents (Norman et al, 1996;Todd & Norman, 2003).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%