1987
DOI: 10.1007/bf00176481
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Opiate reinforcement and naloxone aversion, as revealed by place preference paradigm, in two strains of rats

Abstract: Two strains of rats--LC2-Hi and LC2-Lo--selected for high and low self-stimulation rates, respectively, were tested for responses to opiates and to naloxone using conditioned place preference paradigm. In the two experiments which used opiates as UCS, conditioning was carried out in the non-preferred compartment while in the experiment which used naloxone, conditioning was performed in the preferred compartment. The preference changes were determined on the basis of times spent in the compartments before and a… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

1989
1989
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 27 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Rats learned to associate escalating doses of heroin with a unique chamber while receiving chronic exposure to escalating doses of heroin in their homecages and, when tested during acute spontaneous withdrawal, preferred the heroin-paired chamber. Although conditioned place aversion has been used to examine the aversive state of withdrawal (Azar et al 2003; Dymshitz and Lieblich 1987; Harris and Aston-Jones 1993; Rothwell et al 2009; Spanagel et al 1994), few studies have used conditioned place preference to measure heroin’s incentive value across this relapse-prone period. Paired with chronic homecage exposure to heroin, the present place preference study offers insight into the incentive-motivational value attributed to heroin by a dependent rat, once heroin is discontinued and withdrawal begins.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Rats learned to associate escalating doses of heroin with a unique chamber while receiving chronic exposure to escalating doses of heroin in their homecages and, when tested during acute spontaneous withdrawal, preferred the heroin-paired chamber. Although conditioned place aversion has been used to examine the aversive state of withdrawal (Azar et al 2003; Dymshitz and Lieblich 1987; Harris and Aston-Jones 1993; Rothwell et al 2009; Spanagel et al 1994), few studies have used conditioned place preference to measure heroin’s incentive value across this relapse-prone period. Paired with chronic homecage exposure to heroin, the present place preference study offers insight into the incentive-motivational value attributed to heroin by a dependent rat, once heroin is discontinued and withdrawal begins.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While the magnitude of heroin preference did not correspond to patterns of somatic withdrawal signs, extending limited self-administration analyses (Hellemans et al 2002), it is likely that negative emotional and stress states of withdrawal contributed to heroin preference. In rodents, withdrawal has been associated with increased anxiety- and depression-like behavior (Goeldner et al 2011; Grasing et al 1996; Harris and Aston-Jones 1993; Rothwell et al 2009; Zhang and Schulteis 2008), conditioned place aversions (Dymshitz and Lieblich 1987; Harris and Aston-Jones 1993; Mucha 1987; Rothwell et al 2009; Spanagel et al 1994) elevated self-stimulation thresholds (Schaefer and Michael 1985), and elevated and persistent heroin-seeking behavior (Hutcheson et al 2001; Lenoir and Ahmed 2007; Gerak et al 2009; Negus 2006; Negus and Rice 2009). We speculate that heroin place preference reported here was not driven by avoidance of withdrawal but rather the value attributed to heroin across cycles of withdrawal and re-exposure.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In heroin addicts, drug-related cues can support compulsive drug taking, elicit drug-associated physiological responses, prompt craving and trigger relapse (Childress et al, 1988; O’Brien et al, 1992, 1984; Sherman et al, 1989; Sideroff and Jarvik, 1980; Wikler, 1973). In animal models of addiction, opiate-associated cues can reinforce intravenous drug self-administration (Davis and Smith, 1976; Di Ciano and Everitt, 2004; Dymshitz and Lieblich, 1987), enhance locomotor activity (Mucha et al, 1981), facilitate the acquisition of opiate tolerance (Siegel, 1975), elicit conditioned place preference (Bardo et al, 1984; Bardo and Neisewander, 1986; Schenk et al, 1983) and reinstate drug seeking (McFarland and Ettenberg, 1997; Peck and Ranaldi, 2014; Schuster and Woods, 1968). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition to their ability to establish conditioned taste aversions, opioid antagonists have been shown to be effective unconditioned stimuli for the establishment of conditioned place aversions (10,25). We do not believe such conditioning played a significant role in the present experiment because cage positions of the two diets were reversed daily.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 88%