2008
DOI: 10.2307/20445458
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On the Spatial Metric of Short-SOA Costs of Exogenous Cuing

Abstract: When the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between the cue and the target is short, exogenous spatial cues usually produce a response time benefit. However, consistent with several recent studies, we have found that a short stimulus onset asynchrony is not sufficient. At least one more factor-the number of cue and target locations-also plays a role. Even more interesting, when 8 cue and target locations are used, the effect of an exogenous cue produces a cost on valid cue trials, and the spatial metric of this n… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2008
2008
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
1

Relationship

2
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
(29 reference statements)
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The operational definition of attentional capture is when mean RT to targets in the cued location is less than mean RT to targets in uncued locations. Recently, however, we and others have reported atypical results: a negative cuing effect in which targets can appear in a larger number of display locations than is usually used (Chen, Moore, & Mordkoff, 2008;Gawryszewski, Carreiro, & Magalhaes, 2005;Mordkoff, Halterman, & Chen, 2008). The general goal of the present research is to examine a possible explanation for these atypical findings.…”
mentioning
confidence: 75%
“…The operational definition of attentional capture is when mean RT to targets in the cued location is less than mean RT to targets in uncued locations. Recently, however, we and others have reported atypical results: a negative cuing effect in which targets can appear in a larger number of display locations than is usually used (Chen, Moore, & Mordkoff, 2008;Gawryszewski, Carreiro, & Magalhaes, 2005;Mordkoff, Halterman, & Chen, 2008). The general goal of the present research is to examine a possible explanation for these atypical findings.…”
mentioning
confidence: 75%
“…The present study was motivated by several recent findings of a significant cost of exogenous cuing when the number of display locations was high (e.g., Chen et al, 2008;Gawryszewski et al, 2005). The working hypothesis was that the decrease in the overall validity of the cues, as well as the decrease in the probability that the previous trial involved a valid cue, were jointly responsible for this change in the "standard" result (e.g., Posner & Cohen, 1984).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Finally, it is also worth noting that, when more than three display locations are used, the target can be presented at various distances from the cue on invalid-cue trials (see, e.g., Chen et al, 2008). In the present experiment, whether the target appeared adjacent to or opposite from the cue had no effect on performance with Display Size 4, regardless of whether the previous cue was valid or invalid (see Table 1) (all Fs 1).…”
Section: Control Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several other studies using spatially non-predictive peripheral cues (Chen, Moore, & Mordkoff, 2008;Gawryszewski, Carreiro, & Magalhães, 2005;Prinzmetal, Ha, & Khani, 2010) and spatially predictive central arrow cues (Ceballos, Tivis, Lawton-Craddock, & Nixon, 2005;Geng & Behrmann, 2005) have varied the number of matching target locations and spatial directions between four to eight. However, these studies typically report a single mean RT (or error rate) for valid and invalid trials without accounting for the location of the target in the visual field.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%