1992
DOI: 10.1093/jos/9.1.1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On the Semantics and Pragmatics of Linguistic Feedback

Abstract: This paper is an exploration in the semantics and pragmatics of linguistic feedback, i.e., linguistic mechanisms which enable the participants in spoken interaction to exchange information about basic communicative functions, such as contact, perception, understanding, and attitudinal reactions to the communicated content. Special attention is given to the type of reaction conveyed by feedback utterances, the communicative status of the information conveyed (i. e., the level of awareness and intentionality of … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
160
0
12

Year Published

1998
1998
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 241 publications
(174 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
2
160
0
12
Order By: Relevance
“…That is, although oh diff ers from the other lexical items used in the same slot, i.e., turn-initial before an assessment, because of its symbolic meaning (cf. Fischer, 2000 ), its occurrence in example (1) is similar to examples (13)- (15) in that all markers in this position acknowledge the partner's turn by signalling successful perception and understanding and continued attention, as well as a continuation of the same topic (Allwood, Nivre, & Ahlsén, 1992 ); i.e., they claim that the current utterance relates relevantly to the previous (cf. Fischer, 2010).…”
Section: Gr Ammarmentioning
confidence: 63%
“…That is, although oh diff ers from the other lexical items used in the same slot, i.e., turn-initial before an assessment, because of its symbolic meaning (cf. Fischer, 2000 ), its occurrence in example (1) is similar to examples (13)- (15) in that all markers in this position acknowledge the partner's turn by signalling successful perception and understanding and continued attention, as well as a continuation of the same topic (Allwood, Nivre, & Ahlsén, 1992 ); i.e., they claim that the current utterance relates relevantly to the previous (cf. Fischer, 2010).…”
Section: Gr Ammarmentioning
confidence: 63%
“…' Contrary to common belief, communicative feedback is not merely a way of signalling the interlocutor to continue speaking, but a powerful mechanism that enables listeners to express their mental state towards the speaker's utterance. According to Allwood and colleagues [1,19], communicative feedback signals express the basic communicative functions 1 'contact' (being "willing and able to continue the interaction"), 'perception' (being "willing and able to perceive the message"), 'understanding' (being "willing and able to understand the message"), and 'attitudinal reactions' (being "willing and able to react and (adequately) respond to the message") such as 'acceptance' or 'agreement' [1, p. 3]. These functions are related to each other hierarchically [1,11] such that higher functions imply lower functions (when signalling feedback of type understanding, for example, successful perception and contact are implied) and lower functions block higher functions (e.g., feedback of failed perception entails a problem in understanding).…”
Section: Communicative Feedback As Signals Of Groundingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our attentive speaker agent 'Billie' is therefore equipped with a minimal 'Theory of Mind' that allows it to reason about the mental state that the user was most likely in when producing a feedback signal. This model, which we call 'attributed listener state' (ALS), is conceptualised in terms of the same mental categories that Allwood and colleagues [1,19] assume to underlie the basic communicative functions of feedback.…”
Section: Using Human Feedback In Human-agent Dialoguementioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations