1988
DOI: 10.1016/s0005-7894(88)80003-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On the range of cognitive plasticity in old age as a function of experience: 15 years of intervention research

Abstract: Fifteen years of cognitive research conducted by the first author and his colleagues are summarized. This research has shown that many older adults have the reserve capacity to improve their performance on tests of intelligence and other indicators of cognitive efficacy. Improvement results both from self-guided learning and trainer-guided training in relevant cognitive skills. At the same time, there is increasing evidence that there are aging-related limits in the level of training-based performance attainab… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

7
98
0
3

Year Published

1992
1992
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

4
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 147 publications
(108 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
7
98
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…It involves a component of what in psychological intelligence theory is called fluid intelligence or the mechanical aspects of cognitive functioning. As argued by others and by us (e.g., Baltes & Smith, 1990;Denney, 1990;Perlmutter, 1990), there are other facets of the mind (e.g., knowledge-based domains of functioning such as language comprehension, professional expertise, or wisdom) in which old adults may be equivalent to or better than young adults, even in situations that require maximum use of reserve capacity. Therefore, it is important not to generalize this study's outcome to all domains of the mind.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…It involves a component of what in psychological intelligence theory is called fluid intelligence or the mechanical aspects of cognitive functioning. As argued by others and by us (e.g., Baltes & Smith, 1990;Denney, 1990;Perlmutter, 1990), there are other facets of the mind (e.g., knowledge-based domains of functioning such as language comprehension, professional expertise, or wisdom) in which old adults may be equivalent to or better than young adults, even in situations that require maximum use of reserve capacity. Therefore, it is important not to generalize this study's outcome to all domains of the mind.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…This question is related to the old issue of separating task-specific from task-general effects of training (e.g. Baltes & Lindenberger, 1988;Lövdén et al, 2010). One way to answer this question is to try to determine and evaluate the distance of transfer tasks (Barnett & Ceci, 2002;Noack et al, 2009;Zelinski, 2009;cf.…”
Section: Generality Of Transfermentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Jensen (1998) noted, for example, that that the intelligence factor,g, predicts only around 65% of the variance in its best predictor, matrix reasoning, leaving another 35% that must be governed by other factors, which could alternatively account for observed transfer (see also Shipstead et al, 2012). Thus, if the aim is to make claims about some latent aspect (i.e., a cognitive process or ability) of the transfer task, and if the ultimate "…goal is not to train for the specific task but to impact the construct underlying the task" (McArdle & Prindle, 2008, p. 703; see also Baltes et al, 1986;Baltes & Lindenberger, 1988), then analyses must be…”
Section: Generality Of Transfermentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Baltes & Lindenberger, 1988;Kramer & Willis, 2003;Lindenberger, Kliegl, & Baltes, 1992). Regarding memory plasticity, the general finding is that older adults' plasticity is much more limited than that of younger adults but that cognitively healthy older adults, at least up to their 80s, continue to be able to improve their memory performance through acquiring and using mnemonic techniques.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%