2018
DOI: 10.1037/apl0000304
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On the pursuit of multiple goals with different deadlines.

Abstract: This article presents a theory of how people prioritize their time when pursuing goals with different deadlines. Although progress has been made in understanding the dynamics of multiple-goal pursuit, theory in this area only addresses cases where the goals have the same deadline. We rectify this issue by integrating the multiple-goal pursuit model-a formal theory of multiple goal pursuit-with theories of intertemporal motivation and choice. We examine the ability of four computational models derived from this… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
28
0
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 42 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
1
28
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This suggests participants found it easier to discriminate conflicts from nonconflicts when they were given 20s to make five decisions, than when they were given 8s to make two decisions, with the lower thresholds and reduced availability possibly reflecting some degree of 'satisficing' via a reduction in the depth of processing (e.g., Donkin, Little, & Houpt, 2014). This pattern of results is consistent with research on temporal discounting (e.g., Ballard, Vancouver, & Neal, 2018), showing that individuals tend to work harder when given more immediate deadlines. Our participants likely perceived having 20s to make five decisions to be a less demanding deadline than having 8s to make two decisions, despite having equal time available per decision on average.…”
Section: Trial Loadsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…This suggests participants found it easier to discriminate conflicts from nonconflicts when they were given 20s to make five decisions, than when they were given 8s to make two decisions, with the lower thresholds and reduced availability possibly reflecting some degree of 'satisficing' via a reduction in the depth of processing (e.g., Donkin, Little, & Houpt, 2014). This pattern of results is consistent with research on temporal discounting (e.g., Ballard, Vancouver, & Neal, 2018), showing that individuals tend to work harder when given more immediate deadlines. Our participants likely perceived having 20s to make five decisions to be a less demanding deadline than having 8s to make two decisions, despite having equal time available per decision on average.…”
Section: Trial Loadsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Thus, it is important to reexamine these effects under the conditions in which the assessment of selfregulation success is available. Furthermore, recent research on multiple goal pursuit identified other factors such as different deadlines for active goals (Ballard, Vancouver, & Neal, 2018), and perceived power in terms of ability to control resources required for goalpursuit (Schmid, 2018). Therefore, additive effects of those factors in relation with regulatory fit effects would further our understanding on dynamic resource allocation under multiple goal-pursuit situations.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…On a more positive note, Vancouver and Scherbaum (2008) created computational models of competing theories of self-regulation and used them to develop an empirical study that pitted the theories. Computational models can also be fit to time-series data from single cases (e.g., Vancouver, Weinhardt, et al, 2010) or a set of cases simultaneously (Ballard, Vancouver, & Neal, 2018). Of course, the caveats associated with data fitting that apply to statistical modeling also apply to computational modeling.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A weakness of the IMWM computational model is that we could set parameter values to unrealistic levels to match that trajectory. To validate computational models, studies need to be designed that challenge model components (e.g., Ballard et al, 2018; Vancouver et al, 2016) or help set constraints for the parameters (Forster, 2000).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%