2007
DOI: 10.3758/bf03195946
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On the nature of the affective priming effect: Effects of stimulus onset asynchrony and congruency proportion in naming and evaluative categorization

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
80
1
4

Year Published

2009
2009
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 82 publications
(94 citation statements)
references
References 86 publications
9
80
1
4
Order By: Relevance
“…The relatedness-proportion effect in semantic priming refers to stronger facilitation when the proportion of related primetarget pairs is high rather than low (De Groot, 1984;Neely, Keefe, & Ross 1989;Seidenberg, Waters, Sanders, & Langer 1984). Analogous congruity proportion effects in EP (i.e., enhanced EP when the proportion of congruent pairs is high) were first reported by Klauer and colleagues (Klauer, Mierke, & Musch 2003;Klauer et al, 1997) and were later replicated by Spruyt, Hermans, De Houwer, Vandromme, and Eelen (2007).…”
Section: Strategic Influences On Evaluative Primingmentioning
confidence: 58%
“…The relatedness-proportion effect in semantic priming refers to stronger facilitation when the proportion of related primetarget pairs is high rather than low (De Groot, 1984;Neely, Keefe, & Ross 1989;Seidenberg, Waters, Sanders, & Langer 1984). Analogous congruity proportion effects in EP (i.e., enhanced EP when the proportion of congruent pairs is high) were first reported by Klauer and colleagues (Klauer, Mierke, & Musch 2003;Klauer et al, 1997) and were later replicated by Spruyt, Hermans, De Houwer, Vandromme, and Eelen (2007).…”
Section: Strategic Influences On Evaluative Primingmentioning
confidence: 58%
“…However, while several researchers reported that they were unable to replicate the evaluative priming effect in the absence of dimensional overlap between the prime set and the response set (e.g., De Houwer, Hermans, Rothermund, & Wentura, 2002;Klauer & Musch, Attention please 6 2002;Klinger, Burton, & Pitts, 2000), others reported that they did succeed in capturing this phenomenon, at least under certain conditions (e.g., Bargh, Chaiken, Raymond, & Hymes, 1996; De Houwer, Hermans, & Spruyt, 2001;Everaert, Spruyt & De Houwer, 2011;Hermans, De Houwer, & Eelen, 1994;Spruyt, Hermans, De Houwer, & Eelen, 2002;Spruyt et al, , 2009Spruyt et al, , 2012Spruyt, Hermans, De Houwer, Vandromme, Eelen, 2007;Schmitz & Wentura, 2012;Wentura, 2000;Wentura, & Frings, 2008). These findings suggest that processes other than Stroop-like response interference are also involved in the translation of the outcome of the prime-evaluation process into an observable evaluative priming effect.…”
Section: Attention Pleasementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The finding of significant priming effects with the pronunciation task thus establishes a specific influence of facilitated target access processes on priming effects. Robust priming effects with the pronunciation task have repeatedly been demonstrated for associative prime-target pairs (e.g., Meyer et al, 1975; see Neely, 1991, for a review), whereas for categorically related prime-target pairs (e.g., affective priming), highly inconsistent results have been reported, including congruency, incongruency, Cognitive Processes in Categorical and Associative Priming 8 as well as null effects (e.g., Bargh et al, 1996;De Houwer & Randell, 2004;Glaser & Banaji, 1999;Hermans, De Houwer, & Eelen, 1994; Klauer & Musch, 2001; Schmitz & Wentura, in press;Spruyt, De Houwer, & Hermans, 2009; Spruyt, Hermans, De Houwer, & Eelen, 2002;Spruyt, Hermans, Pandelaere, De Houwer, & Eelen, 2004;Spruyt, Hermans, De Houwer, Vandromme, & Eelen, 2007;Wentura & Frings, 2008; see Klauer & Musch, 2003, andRothermund, 2003, for reviews).…”
Section: Problems Of Previous Attempts To Distinguish Between Mediatimentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The finding of significant priming effects with the pronunciation task thus establishes a specific influence of facilitated target access processes on priming effects. Robust priming effects with the pronunciation task have repeatedly been demonstrated for associative prime-target pairs (e.g., Meyer et al, 1975; see Neely, 1991, for a review), whereas for categorically related prime-target pairs (e.g., affective priming), highly inconsistent results have been reported, including congruency, incongruency, Cognitive Processes in Categorical and Associative Priming 8 as well as null effects (e.g., Bargh et al, 1996;De Houwer & Randell, 2004;Glaser & Banaji, 1999;Hermans, De Houwer, & Eelen, 1994; Klauer & Musch, 2001; Schmitz & Wentura, in press;Spruyt, De Houwer, & Hermans, 2009; Spruyt, Hermans, De Houwer, & Eelen, 2002;Spruyt, Hermans, Pandelaere, De Houwer, & Eelen, 2004;Spruyt, Hermans, De Houwer, Vandromme, & Eelen, 2007;Wentura & Frings, 2008; see Klauer & Musch, 2003, andRothermund, 2003, for reviews).Similarly, using a categorization task that is unrelated to the dimension for which category congruency effects are investigated (e.g., an animacy categorization task is used when testing for affective category priming) should eliminate response competition as a potential explanation of categorical priming effects (but see Schmitz & Wentura, in press).Moreover, since post-lexical mechanisms are most plausible for the lexical decision task (see above) this strategy should also eliminate post-lexical mechanisms. Categorical priming effects are typically eliminated in designs in which the task is to categorize the targets according to another dimension (e.g., De Houwer et al, 2002;Klinger et al, 2000; Klauer & Musch, 2002; but see Spruyt, De Houwer, Hermans, & Eelen, 2007; Schmitz & Wentura, in press).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%