Cerebral Lateralization in Nonhuman Species 1985
DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-286480-3.50009-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On the Inheritance of Direction and Degree of Asymmetry

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3

Citation Types

10
121
0
4

Year Published

1997
1997
2007
2007

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 161 publications
(135 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
10
121
0
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Like handedness in humans, paw preference in mice, as assessed in a food-reaching task, has been widely used as a robust index of functional brain asymmetry to dissect out the genetic and physiological mechanisms underlying lateralization (Cabib et al, 1995;Collins, 1975Collins, , 1985Denenberg et al, 1991;Nielsen et al, 1997;Nosten et al, 1989;Signore et al, 1991b).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Like handedness in humans, paw preference in mice, as assessed in a food-reaching task, has been widely used as a robust index of functional brain asymmetry to dissect out the genetic and physiological mechanisms underlying lateralization (Cabib et al, 1995;Collins, 1975Collins, , 1985Denenberg et al, 1991;Nielsen et al, 1997;Nosten et al, 1989;Signore et al, 1991b).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These criteria correspond to those used by Collins (1991) to select the two lines for the degree of lateralization. The initial studies in genetically uniform mice have shown that these measures of paw preferences in mice are enduring and not task specific (Betancur et al, 1991;Biddle and Eales, 1996;Collins, 1985;Nosten et al, 1989; Signore et al, 1991a, b). …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…D. Hopkins, Bales, & Bennett, 1993;Kubota, 1990;Matoba, Masataka, & Tanioka, 1991;Westergaard & Suomi, 1997), and in none of these studies can potential genetic factors be isolated from environmental factors, because the offspring in most of these studies were raised by their biological mothers and, therefore, no specific manipulation of rearing environment was performed. Selectivebreeding studies in mice clearly suggest no genetic basis for the determination of directional biases in paw preferences, although strength of paw preference appears to be under some genetic control (see Collins, 1985, for a review).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The pervasiveness of human right-handedness has led to numerous debates about the mechanisms involved in the expression of hand preference (B. Hopkins & Ronnqvist, 1998). Both genetic models (Annett, 1985;Corballis, 1997;Laland, Kumm, Van Horn, & Feldman, 1995;McManus, 1985;Yeo & Gangestad, 1993) and environmental models (Collins, 1985;Provins, 1997) have been proposed to explain the origin of human hand preference. The principal data in support of a genetic basis for hand preference are that it runs in families (Curt, De Agostini, Maccario, & Dellatolas, 1995;Laland et al, 1995;McGee & Cozad, 1980;McManus & Bryden, 1992) and that offspring typically exhibit patterns of hand preference more similar to their biological parents compared with offspring who have been adopted or raised by stepparents (Carter-Saltzman, 1980;Hicks & Kinsbourne, 1976).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Explaining the pervasive degree of right-handedness in the human species has fostered considerable theoretical debate and empirical investigation. Genetic, biological, and environmental models have been proposed to explain this population-level right-handedness, each with varying amounts of empirical support (Annett, 1999;Collins, 1985;Corballis, 1997;Laland, Kumm, Van Horn, & Feldman, 1995;Previc, 1991;Provins, 1997;Trevarthen, 1996). Equally important are theoretical models proposed to explain the occurrences of left-handedness or non-right-handedness in human populations.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%