The platform will undergo maintenance on Sep 14 at about 7:45 AM EST and will be unavailable for approximately 2 hours.
1982
DOI: 10.1056/nejm198205273062103
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On the Elicitation of Preferences for Alternative Therapies

Abstract: We investigated how variations in the way information is presented to patients influence their choices between alternative therapies. Data were presented summarizing the results of surgery and radiation therapy for lung cancer to 238 ambulatory patients with different chronic medical conditions and to 491 graduate students and 424 physicians. We asked the subjects to imagine that they had lung cancer and to choose between the two therapies on the basis of both cumulative probabilities and life-expectancy data.… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

29
644
6
17

Year Published

1993
1993
2009
2009

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 1,420 publications
(703 citation statements)
references
References 4 publications
29
644
6
17
Order By: Relevance
“…As a consequence of the process of reconciliation with the treatment decision the preferences presented here may be too positive in the patients undergoing chemotherapy and too negative in the patients not scheduled for chemotherapy. Studies into treatment preferences have been carried out in patients who have already undergone the particular treatment (Nieuwkerk et al, 1998;Ravdin et al, 1998;Silvestri et al, 1998), in mixed patient groups with some patients having experience and others not (O'Connor, 1989;Kiebert et al, 1993;Yellen et al, 1994;McQuellon et al, 1995;Yellen and Cella, 1995;Cullen et al, 1996;Elit et al, 1996;Brundage et al, 1997;Lindley et al, 1998), in patients who were about to undergo the specific treatment (Slevin et al, 1990;Palda et al, 1997), or in respondents who did not receive the treatment under concern (McNeil et al, 1982;Llewellyn-Thomas et al, 1996). As a result, all studies mentioned above have been carried out in a hypothetical situation and may have been influenced by the effect of 'reconciliation with the treatment decision'.…”
Section: Limitations and Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As a consequence of the process of reconciliation with the treatment decision the preferences presented here may be too positive in the patients undergoing chemotherapy and too negative in the patients not scheduled for chemotherapy. Studies into treatment preferences have been carried out in patients who have already undergone the particular treatment (Nieuwkerk et al, 1998;Ravdin et al, 1998;Silvestri et al, 1998), in mixed patient groups with some patients having experience and others not (O'Connor, 1989;Kiebert et al, 1993;Yellen et al, 1994;McQuellon et al, 1995;Yellen and Cella, 1995;Cullen et al, 1996;Elit et al, 1996;Brundage et al, 1997;Lindley et al, 1998), in patients who were about to undergo the specific treatment (Slevin et al, 1990;Palda et al, 1997), or in respondents who did not receive the treatment under concern (McNeil et al, 1982;Llewellyn-Thomas et al, 1996). As a result, all studies mentioned above have been carried out in a hypothetical situation and may have been influenced by the effect of 'reconciliation with the treatment decision'.…”
Section: Limitations and Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…13 More recent evidence indicates that loss-framed messages appear to be successful in promoting behaviours related to early detection, such as clinical skin examinations, 14 mammography screening, 15 breast self examination, 13 and HIV testing. 16 In contrast, gain-framed messages have been found to influence prevention related behaviours such as sunscreen use, 17 preference for surgical procedures, [18][19][20][21] and engaging in regular physical exercise. 22 Because quitting smoking is a preventive health behaviour, these studies suggest that messages designed to encourage smokers to quit might be more effective if they were framed in terms of gains rather than losses.…”
Section: Utilise Gain-framed Messagesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast to the lack of studies on treatment choice, several studies have been performed on the nature and background of treatment preference (Cassileth et al, 1980;McNeil et al, 1982;Llewellyn-Thomas et al, 1995;Flood et al, 1996;Brundage et al, 1997;Mazur and Hickam, 1997;Lindley et al, 1998;Silvestri et al, 1998;Stalmeier and Bezembinder, 1999;Stiggelbout and de Haes, 2001). In the literature, the patient's preference for treatment is seen and used as an indication for their actual treatment choice.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Preferences for treatment are thus usually assessed after patients have received information concerning their treatment options (McNeil et al, 1982;Flood et al, 1996;O'Connor, 1998;Lindley et al, 1998;Silvestri et al, 1998). However, the patient, being a modern consumer of health care, is not a tabula rasa.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%