2020
DOI: 10.1007/s40881-020-00094-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On the effect of anchoring on valuations when the anchor is transparently uninformative

Abstract: We test whether anchoring affects people’s elicited valuations for a bottle of wine in individual decision-making and in markets. We anchor subjects by asking them if they are willing to sell a bottle of wine for a transparently uninformative random price. We elicit subjects’ Willingness-To-Accept for the bottle before and after the market. Subjects participate in a double auction market either in a small or a large trading group. The variance in subjects’ Willingness-To-Accept shrinks within trading groups. O… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
(26 reference statements)
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Some studies suggest possible boundary conditions moderating the effect. The anchoring effect on valuation has been shown to be stronger in hypothetical valuations than in valuations with real-world consequences (Brzozowicz & Krawczyk, 2022;Jung et al, 2016), stronger when the anchor is perceived to be informative than when it is perceived to be uninformative (Ioannidis et al, 2020;Li et al, 2021), and stronger in buying contexts (i.e., willingness-to-pay judgments) than in selling contexts (i.e., willingness-to-accept judgments; Li et al, 2021).…”
Section: Valuation and Purchasing Decisionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some studies suggest possible boundary conditions moderating the effect. The anchoring effect on valuation has been shown to be stronger in hypothetical valuations than in valuations with real-world consequences (Brzozowicz & Krawczyk, 2022;Jung et al, 2016), stronger when the anchor is perceived to be informative than when it is perceived to be uninformative (Ioannidis et al, 2020;Li et al, 2021), and stronger in buying contexts (i.e., willingness-to-pay judgments) than in selling contexts (i.e., willingness-to-accept judgments; Li et al, 2021).…”
Section: Valuation and Purchasing Decisionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, while subjects can be biased toward choosing one brand of beverage over others, this occurs only when they are thirsty [118]. Similarly, anchoring effects are found only when the anchor is presented as informative [119,120]. It is accordingly rather difficult to generalize from such special-case results.…”
Section: Box 2 Key Criticisms Of the Evidence For Unconscious Effectsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, they concluded that the strength and robustness of the phenomenon might not be as great as previously believed. Ioannidis et al [ 45 ], in the recent paper questioning the robustness of the anchoring effect on preferences, observed no effect of anchors on the valuation of a bottle of wine. They also performed a concise meta-analysis and demonstrated that the anchoring effect is weaker for familiar goods than for unfamiliar ones and also observed a null result of anchoring in studies with clearly uninformative anchors.…”
Section: Literature Reviewmentioning
confidence: 99%