1998
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.24.6.1379
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On the difference between strength-based and frequency-based mirror effects in recognition memory.

Abstract: A mirror effect can be produced by manipulating word class (e.g., high vs. low frequency) or by manipulating strength (e.g., short vs. long study time). The results of 5 experiments reported here suggest that a strength-based mirror effect is caused by a shift in the location of the decision criterion, whereas a frequency-based mirror effect occurs although the criterion remains fixed with respect to word frequency. Evidence supporting these claims is provided by a series of studies in which high frequency (HF… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

41
382
13
5

Year Published

2002
2002
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 208 publications
(447 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
41
382
13
5
Order By: Relevance
“…We used a paired-samples t test to compare the recognition scores (hits -false alarms) for words that had been studied either once or three times. In line with the extensive repetition literature (e.g., Bentin & Moscovitch, 1988;Murnane & Shiffrin, 1991;Ratcliff, Clark, & Shiffrin, 1990;Stretch & Wixted, 1998), participants recognized items that had been studied three times significantly better than items studied only once, t(23)=5.76, p<.001. Separate t tests demonstrated that there were (1) more hits, t(23)=5.59, p<.001, and (2) fewer false alarms, t(23)=2.19, p<.05, for words presented three times than for words presented once.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 60%
“…We used a paired-samples t test to compare the recognition scores (hits -false alarms) for words that had been studied either once or three times. In line with the extensive repetition literature (e.g., Bentin & Moscovitch, 1988;Murnane & Shiffrin, 1991;Ratcliff, Clark, & Shiffrin, 1990;Stretch & Wixted, 1998), participants recognized items that had been studied three times significantly better than items studied only once, t(23)=5.76, p<.001. Separate t tests demonstrated that there were (1) more hits, t(23)=5.59, p<.001, and (2) fewer false alarms, t(23)=2.19, p<.05, for words presented three times than for words presented once.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 60%
“…Such a pattern of results (i.e., that higher hit-rates are often accompanied by lower false-alarm rates) has been dubbed the mirror effect by Glanzer and colleagues (Glanzer & Adams, 1985;Glanzer, Adams, Iverson, & Kim, 1993). Although the mirror effect is more general than manipulations of word frequency (e.g., encoding manipulations such as increased study time also produce mirror effects; Hirshman, 1995;Ratcliff, Clark, & Shiffrin, 1990;Stretch & Wixted, 1998), accounting for the word-frequency mirror effect has proven especially difficult for many theories of recognition memory. In particular, the word-frequency mirror effect has been difficult for global matching models (Gillund & Shiffrin, 1984;Hintzman, 1988;Murdock, 1982) to explain.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The approach takes inspiration from Bayesian models of recognition memory, especially from McClelland and Chappell's subjective likelihood model. 16 One of the most significant differences between the approach presented here and existing Bayesian models 8,12,16,17 is that we obtain a mirror effect without an explicit likelihood ratio calculation. We have shown here that the two basic classes of mirror effects can be generated from a simple, competitive learning neural network in which the values for the familiarity axis are directly generated from the activation of the winning detectors.…”
Section: Conclusion and Further Workmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is based on the observation that participants do not appreciate that low frequency items are more memorable, but instead adopt a single response criterion/threshold, even if provided with explicit cues about the item type. 8 …”
Section: Our Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation