1993
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.2420230207
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On the clarity of expressive and contextual information in the recognition of emotions: A methodological critique

Abstract: Ekman, Friesen and Ellsworth's (19826) influential review on emotional expression and context prescribed the current standard criteria for selecting clear and comparable faces and contexts. These recommendations might not pay enough attention to the peculiarities of contextual information. Criteria of clarity for contextual stimuli should be grounded in the process of categorization of social situations (e.g. prototypicality and salience). A set of criteria of contextual clarity were designed according to thi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
20
0

Year Published

1994
1994
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
1
20
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This general finding was replicated using bodily and situational information conveyed by a visual scene (Munn, 1940), film clips (Goldberg, 1951), and video of the actual eliciting circumstances (Sherman, 1927). More recently, this finding has received further support (Fernandez-Dols, Sierra, & Ruiz-Belda, 1993; Fernandez-Dols, Wallbot & Sanchez, 1991, Study 2;Spignesi & Shor, 1981; Wallbott, 1988a, Study 2;Wallbott, 1988b). For example, portrayals of anger are more likely to be perceived as fearful when paired with the description of a dangerous situation (Carroll & Russell, 1996, Study 1).…”
supporting
confidence: 58%
“…This general finding was replicated using bodily and situational information conveyed by a visual scene (Munn, 1940), film clips (Goldberg, 1951), and video of the actual eliciting circumstances (Sherman, 1927). More recently, this finding has received further support (Fernandez-Dols, Sierra, & Ruiz-Belda, 1993; Fernandez-Dols, Wallbot & Sanchez, 1991, Study 2;Spignesi & Shor, 1981; Wallbott, 1988a, Study 2;Wallbott, 1988b). For example, portrayals of anger are more likely to be perceived as fearful when paired with the description of a dangerous situation (Carroll & Russell, 1996, Study 1).…”
supporting
confidence: 58%
“…Clarity Clarity refers to the amount and quality of the emotional information available to the perceiver (Ekman, Friesen, & Ellsworth, 1982;Fernandez-Dols, Sierra, & Ruiz-Belda, 1993). Thus, clarity is fundamental to the perception of facial expressions as well as to achieve mutual adjustment between people (e.g., Bach, Buxtorf, Grandjean, & Strik, 2009).…”
Section: Dimensions Of Interestmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Here, we introduce a new methodological criterion that should be considered: the analysis of data using confusion matrices and the aggregation of data across meaningful emotion combinations. Many studies (Aviezer et al., 2008; Carroll & Russell, 1996; Fernandez‐Dols et al., 1993; Knudsen & Muzekari, 1983; Russell & Fehr, 1987) ask observers to judge emotions by selecting an emotion term from a list (e.g., anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise). Yet, no study to date reports a full confusion matrix of the data with the percentage of observers selecting each of the response alternatives vis‐à‐vis each face‐context combination.…”
Section: Methodological Issuesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Others have advocated an additive effect , suggesting that judgments of emotion are more accurate and reliable when information about context is incorporated (Aviezer et al., 2008; Bruner & Tagiuri, 1954; Knudsen & Muzekari, 1983; Meeren, van Heijnsbergen, & de Gelder, 2005). Some have argued for none of these (Fernandez‐Dols, Sierra, & Ruiz‐Belda, 1993; Goodenough & Tinker, 1931; Munn, 1940; Vinacke, 1949).…”
Section: Judging Faces In Contextmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation