1996
DOI: 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00679-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On the bases of two subtypes of development dyslexia

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

53
555
12
31

Year Published

1997
1997
2010
2010

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 404 publications
(651 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
53
555
12
31
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition, the proportion of hard phonological and surface dyslexics was consistently across studies almost the same (15% vs. 17% in Castles & Coltheart, 1993;9% vs. 12% in Stanovich et al, 1997; 10% in both cases in Manis et al, 1996). A similar result was observed in the latency-based French study (16% vs. 19%, respectively, in Sprenger-Charolles et al, 2000), but not in the accuracy-based French studies, where the percentage of hard phonological dyslexics was found to be lower than that of hard surface dyslexics (3% vs. 23% in Génard et al, 1998;10% vs. 19% in Sprenger-Charolles et al, 2000).…”
Section: Results Of Multiple-case Studiesmentioning
confidence: 71%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In addition, the proportion of hard phonological and surface dyslexics was consistently across studies almost the same (15% vs. 17% in Castles & Coltheart, 1993;9% vs. 12% in Stanovich et al, 1997; 10% in both cases in Manis et al, 1996). A similar result was observed in the latency-based French study (16% vs. 19%, respectively, in Sprenger-Charolles et al, 2000), but not in the accuracy-based French studies, where the percentage of hard phonological dyslexics was found to be lower than that of hard surface dyslexics (3% vs. 23% in Génard et al, 1998;10% vs. 19% in Sprenger-Charolles et al, 2000).…”
Section: Results Of Multiple-case Studiesmentioning
confidence: 71%
“…More recently, five multiple-case studies were carried out, three with English-speaking dyslexics (Castles & Coltheart, 1993;Manis, Seidenberg, Doi, McBride-Chang, & Peterson, 1996;Stanovich, Siegel, & Gottardo, 1997) 1 and two with French-speaking dyslexics (Génard, Mousty, Content, Alegria, Leybaert, & Morais, 1998;Sprenger-Charolles et al, 2000). Two methods were used to analyze the results: the classical method and the regression method.…”
Section: Results Of Multiple-case Studiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The remaining 35 children (mean age 8.3 ± 0.4 years; 21 girls) all reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision and had an estimated IQ of ≥85 (block design test of the HAWIK-III) (Tewes, et al 1999). The children (n=35) were grouped into normal, ageappropriate (NR; >40 th percentile; n=18, mean age 8.3 ± 0.4; 11 girls) (Manis et al, 1996;Shaywitz et al, 2003) or poor readers (PR; <25th percentile; n=14, mean age 8.3 ± 0.3; 7 girls) (Rutherford, 2006;Shaywitz et al, 2002) according to their reading performance (number of correctly read words per minute, subtest of Salzburger Lesetest: SLT) (Landerl et al, 1997). The three remaining children had reading scores between the 25th and the 40th percentile (gap group, 2 girls) and were only included in regression analyses.…”
Section: Study Design and Subjectsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Two critical dimensions that may be important are the degree of phonological deficit and the degree of language impairment (Gallagher, Frith, & Snowling, 2000;Griffiths & Snowling, 2002;Joanisse et al, 2000;Manis, Seidenberg, Doi, McBride-Chang, & Petersen, 1996;Stanovich, Siegel, & Gottardo, 1997). According to the phonological representations hypothesis, the degree of phonological impairment should be the primary variable affecting gating task performance.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%