1994
DOI: 10.1111/j.1468-0017.1994.tb00227.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On Being Systematically Connectionist

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
37
0

Year Published

1998
1998
2009
2009

Publication Types

Select...
3
3
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 49 publications
(37 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
0
37
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, an extension of the RAAM networks of Pollack (1990) that was proposed by Chalmers (1990) and Niklasson and Gelder (1994) uses two separate networks, one to encode (and decode) all possible instances of variable in a fixed input bank, and another to transform the encoded representations in a systematic way; the structure of these models precisely parallels the division between encoding and computation in standard symbolic models. Such networks are best seen as implementations of those models, not as genuine, nonsymbolic alternatives.…”
Section: Alternative Architecturesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, an extension of the RAAM networks of Pollack (1990) that was proposed by Chalmers (1990) and Niklasson and Gelder (1994) uses two separate networks, one to encode (and decode) all possible instances of variable in a fixed input bank, and another to transform the encoded representations in a systematic way; the structure of these models precisely parallels the division between encoding and computation in standard symbolic models. Such networks are best seen as implementations of those models, not as genuine, nonsymbolic alternatives.…”
Section: Alternative Architecturesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Frank (2006) noted that the models by Bodén (2004) and Hadley et al (2001) only managed to behave systematically because they were specifically tailored for that purpose. Likewise, the systematic connectionist model proposed by Niklasson and van Gelder (1994) was criticized by Hadley (1994a) and Phillips (1998) for depending on hand-crafted input representations that explicitly encoded syntactic class information. In addition, Haselager and van Rappard (1998) remarked that the model required a very extensive and carefully arranged training regime.…”
Section: Demonstration Versus Explanationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A connectionist model that performs such operations could therefore be considered an implementation of a symbol system. For instance, the Recursive Auto-Associative Memory (RAAM) models proposed by Chalmers (1990) and Niklasson and van Gelder (1994), consist of two networks: The first learns to encode the possible instantiations of a variable, and the second performs transformations over the resulting representations. This architecture, so Marcus (1998b) argues, 'precisely parallels the division between encoding and computation in standard symbolic models ' (p. 270) and is therefore not a relevant counterexample to Fodor and Pylyshyn's (1988) claims.…”
Section: Implementation Of a Symbol Systemmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the classical RAAM and its variants, systematicity is based on the properties of the neural code; similar concepts are represented by topologically close vectors, which enables relevant generalization (Pollack, 1990;Niklasson and van Gelder, 1994). Bodén and Niklasson (2000) recently proposed an extension of RAAM that learns the properties of objects, based on their relations within the training set.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, the RAAM has been used to perform holistic computations, e.g. active-passive transformations of grammatical phrases (Chalmers, 1990;Chrisman, 1991), or logical transformations of symbolic expressions (Niklasson and van Gelder, 1994). Niklasson and van Gelder noted that holistic processes in RAAM are sensitive to structure, and therefore address Fodor and Pylyshyn's second argument.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%