Our system is currently under heavy load due to increased usage. We're actively working on upgrades to improve performance. Thank you for your patience.
2007
DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.294
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Olfactory-induced synesthesias: A review and model.

Abstract: Recent reviews of synesthesia concentrate upon rare neurodevelopmental examples and exclude common olfactory-induced experiences with which they may profitably be compared. Like the neurodevelopmental synesthesias, odor-induced experiences involve different sensory modalities; are reliable, asymmetric (concurrents cannot induce), and automatic; and the inducer-concurrent relationship is learnt. Unlike neurodevelopmental synesthesias, these experiences are universal and their synesthetic nature goes unrecognize… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

4
52
1

Year Published

2010
2010
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 83 publications
(57 citation statements)
references
References 164 publications
4
52
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Similar reasoning might explain why rare shape-taste correspondences are classified as synaesthetic, but not taste-smell associations (e.g. the 'sweet' smell of chocolate (Stevenson & Tomiczek, 2007;van Campen, 2008) which naturally belong together and are thus highly predictable. This perspective of ignored or discounted concurrents predicts that awareness of visuallyevoked sounds might be promoted in some individuals if attention is drawn to them under verifiably silent conditions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…Similar reasoning might explain why rare shape-taste correspondences are classified as synaesthetic, but not taste-smell associations (e.g. the 'sweet' smell of chocolate (Stevenson & Tomiczek, 2007;van Campen, 2008) which naturally belong together and are thus highly predictable. This perspective of ignored or discounted concurrents predicts that awareness of visuallyevoked sounds might be promoted in some individuals if attention is drawn to them under verifiably silent conditions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…A benefit of the present account comes, then, from proposing that attributions of "sweetness" (Stevenson & Boakes, 2004), pitch elevation (Belkin et al, 1997;Crisinel & Spence, 2012b), hue (Gilbert, Martin, & Kemp, 1996), brightness (Schiller, 1935), or shape (Hanson-Vaux et al, 2013; can be explained in terms of perceptual processes grounded in exposure and structural determinants, instead of being divided between "synesthetic" connections, on the one hand (Stevenson & Tomiczek, 2007), and conceptual or metaphorical transfers, on the other. It also crucially expands consideration of the crossmodal associations elicited by olfactory cues beyond the idiosyncratic evocations studied as the "Proust phenomenon" (e.g., Chu & Downes, 2002;Willander & Larsson, 2006.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The final section concludes with two arguments related to seeking a better understanding of the crossmodal correspondences holding between contingent features, such as those holding between smells and auditory or shape features: First, researchers should resist the idea that the associations are merely anecdotal, but rather should subject them to systematic investigation (see also Stevenson et al, 2012). Second, we propose an alternative framework to investigate the most surprising cases of crossmodal associations, one that contrasts with their being seen as either synesthetic connections (an interpretation that was suggested for smelltaste associations by Stevenson & Tomiczek, 2007, and is easily extended to crossmodal associations between smell and musical notes) or merely grounded in episodic memories (as has been proposed to explain "Proustian" associations between odors and visual images; see Chu & Downes, 2002;Willander & Larsson, 2006.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We live in a multisensory world, and our brains constantly combine information from different sensory modalities in order to make sense of our environment (see Calvert, Spence, & Stein, 2004). The senses of taste and smell are so tightly combined in the evaluation of flavor that it is sometimes considered a form of synesthesia that is common to us all (Auvray & Spence, 2008;Small & Prescott, 2005;Stevenson & Tomiczek, 2007; see also Djordjevic, Zatorre, & Jones-Gotman, 2004). Moreover, the evaluation of the sweetness (i.e., a gustatory property) of a novel odor can be modified simply by pairing it during training with a sweet taste (Stevenson, Boakes, & Prescott, 1998).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%