2008
DOI: 10.1037/a0012766
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Oh, honey, I already forgot that: Strategic control of directed forgetting in older and younger adults.

Abstract: Two experiments investigated list-method directed forgetting with older and younger adults. Using standard directed forgetting instructions, significant forgetting was obtained with younger but not older adults. However, in Experiment 1 older adults showed forgetting with an experimenterprovided strategy that induced a mental context change --specifically, engaging in diversionary thought. Experiment 2 showed that age related differences in directed forgetting occurred because older adults were less likely tha… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
58
5

Year Published

2009
2009
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 50 publications
(70 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
(131 reference statements)
7
58
5
Order By: Relevance
“…The one instruction (sometimes called the "whoops" procedure; see the Method section) provided a comprehensible rationale for the (unexpected) forget cue and placed high emphasis on the need to forget (high-emphasis condition); the other instruction did not provide a rationale for the forget cue and emphasis on the need to forget was relatively low (low-emphasis condition). On the basis of previous work (e.g., Sahakyan, Delaney, & Goodmon, 2008), we expected that the wording of the forget instruction would not affect young adults' performance, and that adults would show reliable DF in both the high-emphasis and the low-emphasis conditions. If young children suffered from a fundamental deficiency in the required processes, efficient DF should be absent in both the high-emphasis and low-emphasis condition.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The one instruction (sometimes called the "whoops" procedure; see the Method section) provided a comprehensible rationale for the (unexpected) forget cue and placed high emphasis on the need to forget (high-emphasis condition); the other instruction did not provide a rationale for the forget cue and emphasis on the need to forget was relatively low (low-emphasis condition). On the basis of previous work (e.g., Sahakyan, Delaney, & Goodmon, 2008), we expected that the wording of the forget instruction would not affect young adults' performance, and that adults would show reliable DF in both the high-emphasis and the low-emphasis conditions. If young children suffered from a fundamental deficiency in the required processes, efficient DF should be absent in both the high-emphasis and low-emphasis condition.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It has been argued that inhibition is needed to suppress previously, but no longer, relevant items from working memory so as to minimize interference within these tasks (Friedman & Miyake, 2004;Hasher et al, 1999;Kane, Conway, Hambrick, & Engle, 2007). Furthermore, numerous studies suggest that the ability to delete previously attended information shows a marked decline with age, as older adults' performance on these tasks is greatly improved by conditions that minimize the build-up of proactive interference (e.g., Lustig, May, & Hasher, 2001;May et al, 1999;Rowe, Hasher, & Turcotte, 2008Sahakyan, Delaney, & Goodmon, 2008).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As we were interested in the latter form of cognitive control, we used the list method in our study. The list method (e.g., MacLeod, 1998MacLeod, , 1999Zellner & Bäuml, 2006) assesses cognitive control either through intentional efforts to remember and forget (see also Bjork, Bjork, & Anderson, 1998) or through efforts to change one's mental context (Sahakyan, Delaney, & Goodmon, 2008;Sahakyan & Foster, 2009;Sahakyan & Kelley, 2002). More specifically, it requires an intentional effort to control what is forgotten and what is remembered: participants must voluntarily not recall TBF items and voluntarily recall the TBR items (Bjork et al, 1998;Power et al, 2000).…”
Section: Directed Forgettingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…memories (Dywan & Murphy, 1996;Hasher et al, 1999;Jacoby et al, 2005;Zacks et al, 1996;but see Castel, Farb, & Craik, 2007;Sahakyan et al, 2008). Alternatively, in terms of negative material, recent researchers have found what they term, a "positivity effect" (Charles et al, 2003;Mather & Carstensen, 2005;): older adults are better able to control and forget negative material, thereby facilitating emotional well-being (see also Castel et al, 2007).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation