2023
DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2023.06.047
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Obstacle avoidance in aerial pursuit

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

1
2
0

Year Published

2024
2024
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
2

Relationship

1
1

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
1
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The birds turned tightly around the near corner of the tunnel aperture, aiming at an entry point located at φ ≈ ±0.3 and hence approximately 0.1 m from the near side of the tunnel aperture. This behaviour left them with a clearance of approximately one wing length from the tunnel wall ( figure 3 a ), consistent with results from Harris’ hawks, which also aim for a clearance of approximately one wing length when steering around obstacles [ 19 ]. It follows that our observations do not support the hypothesis that zebra finches centre their flight by balancing the optic flow between their left and right visual hemispheres upon entering a tunnel.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 85%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The birds turned tightly around the near corner of the tunnel aperture, aiming at an entry point located at φ ≈ ±0.3 and hence approximately 0.1 m from the near side of the tunnel aperture. This behaviour left them with a clearance of approximately one wing length from the tunnel wall ( figure 3 a ), consistent with results from Harris’ hawks, which also aim for a clearance of approximately one wing length when steering around obstacles [ 19 ]. It follows that our observations do not support the hypothesis that zebra finches centre their flight by balancing the optic flow between their left and right visual hemispheres upon entering a tunnel.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 85%
“…Several studies have modelled obstacle avoidance in birds and insects as a gap negotiation behaviour [11,[13][14][15], which means that rather than treating obstacles as repellers in a sense-and-avoid strategy, as is typically used in engineering contexts [19], the clearance between them is treated as an attractor [11,[13][14][15] in a sense-and-approach strategy. While the two treatments might be functionally equivalent under certain circumstances, they differ mechanistically-particularly in relation to the sensory cues that they may be expected to rely upon.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation