1981
DOI: 10.3758/bf03212035
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Object permanence in cats and dogs

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
98
4

Year Published

2003
2003
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 93 publications
(108 citation statements)
references
References 12 publications
4
98
4
Order By: Relevance
“…That is, they successfully located a treat that moved away from the bottom of the tube after falling from it toward the far end of the box. Although some studies suggest that dogs do not understand invisible displacement problems (CollierBaker et al 2004;, a number of other studies suggest that they do understand these problems (e.g., Triana and Pasnak 1981;Doré 1992, 1993;Watson et al 2001). Perhaps dogs understand invisible displacement when it is achieved via gravity or self-propelled motion but not if it is brought about through the manipulations of human actors in a more artiWcial manner.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…That is, they successfully located a treat that moved away from the bottom of the tube after falling from it toward the far end of the box. Although some studies suggest that dogs do not understand invisible displacement problems (CollierBaker et al 2004;, a number of other studies suggest that they do understand these problems (e.g., Triana and Pasnak 1981;Doré 1992, 1993;Watson et al 2001). Perhaps dogs understand invisible displacement when it is achieved via gravity or self-propelled motion but not if it is brought about through the manipulations of human actors in a more artiWcial manner.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Researchers have begun to explore dogs' knowledge of objects in a variety of ways. For example, several past studies successfully excluding the role of olfactory cues have demonstrated that dogs can succeed at invisible displacement problems (e.g., Triana and Pasnak 1981;Doré 1992, 1993;Watson et al 2001; but see CollierBaker et al 2004;. Additionally, dogs do show an initial gravity bias, and yet they (unlike human infants and monkeys) are able to learn to overcome their gravity bias (Osthaus et al 2003).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some authors take the view that convincing evidence from a single individual is sufficient to demonstrate that a given cognitive trait is within the capacity of the species [12]. Although true in a trivial sense, this perspective imposes two important limitations on the field.…”
Section: Limitations Of the 'Cognitive Capacity' Perspectivementioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, testing across a wide range of species has failed to reveal consistent evidence of success on the task in most other animals, including dolphins (Doré, Goulet, & Herman, 1991), numerous species of monkeys (e.g., de Blois et al, 1998;Natale & Antinucci, 1989), cats (e.g., Doré, 1986;Dumas, 1992), hamsters (Thinus-Blanc & Scardigli, 1981), and domestic chickens (É tienne, 1973). However, striking exceptions to this pattern of findings have come from research with dogs (e.g., Gagnon & Doré, 1992, 1993, 1994Triana & Pasnak, 1981) and psittacine birds (e.g., Pepperberg & Funk, 1990;Pepperberg & Kozak, 1986;Pepperberg, Willner, & Gravitz, 1997). In two longitudinal studies, puppies (Gagnon & Doré, 1994) and an African grey parrot (Pepperberg et al, 1997) were shown to progress through a sequence of stages on displacement tasks similar to those of human infants.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%