2013
DOI: 10.3758/s13423-013-0449-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Not just the norm: Exemplar-based models also predict face aftereffects

Abstract: The face recognition literature has considered two competing accounts of how faces are represented within the visual system: Exemplar-based models assume that faces are represented via their similarity to exemplars of previously experienced faces, while norm-based models assume that faces are represented with respect to their deviation from an average face, or norm. Face identity aftereffects have been taken as compelling evidence in favor of a norm-based account over an exemplar-based account. After a relativ… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
63
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 51 publications
(66 citation statements)
references
References 74 publications
3
63
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, Ross et al (2014) have shown that both two-pool opponent coding (norm-based) and exemplar (cf. non-norm-based, multichannel) models can account for a range of aftereffect results, although they did not consider the precise pattern of increase across the full natural range seen here.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Ross et al (2014) have shown that both two-pool opponent coding (norm-based) and exemplar (cf. non-norm-based, multichannel) models can account for a range of aftereffect results, although they did not consider the precise pattern of increase across the full natural range seen here.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Typically, two-pool models have been employed to account for what appears to be opponent processing (e.g. Over, 1971) and have been successfully Consistently, many face after-effects are found to be larger for strong (e.g., extremely large or unusual) than weak adaptors indicating norm-based coding for many facial attributes (Burton, Jeffery, Skinner, Benton, & Rhodes, 2013 results have recently been modeled (Ross et al, 2014) indicating that an exemplar-based model of face-space can account for after-effects.…”
Section: Facial Adaptationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This raises the possibility that decision-level processes may interact with adaptation-induced perceptual changes, perhaps in non-obvious ways. In a model of face identification decisions, simulations based on both multichannel and opponent-channel encodings of high dimensional face spaces predicted greater shifts in identity boundaries along stimulus trajectories that span an average face than along trajectories that do not (Ross et al, 2013). These simulations suggest that this paradigm is not diagnostic.…”
Section: Larger Shifts In Category Boundaries Along Stimulus Trajementioning
confidence: 87%
“…Alternatively, locations in face space might be encoded by multiple channels, each relatively narrowly-tuned to a sub-region of face space, analogous to the channels thought to underlie the perception of orientation (Giese & Leopold, 2005;Robbins et al, 2007;Ross, Deroche, & Palmeri, 2013;Wallis, 2013;Webster & MacLeod, 2011;Zhao, Seriès, Hancock, & Bednar, 2011). According to this proposal, any dimension of facial variance is encoded by many channels, each responding maximally to some particular value along the dimension and less to values 'higher' or 'lower' than it, with the average face having no unique status.…”
Section: Adaptation To Faces and Other Complex Objects 30mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation