2009
DOI: 10.1016/j.erap.2008.09.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Norme francophone de complétion de trigrammes chez des participants âgés de 30 à 93 ans

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…A high variability in responses was observed in the study of Martin, Dressaire, Guerdoux, Trouillet, Brouillet, Brouillet, and Maury (2009), which used unconstrained stems of French words. In 3,276 different participant answers, only one response achieved a degree of agreement of 60 %, and only 18 responses obtained a degree of agreement above 50 %.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…A high variability in responses was observed in the study of Martin, Dressaire, Guerdoux, Trouillet, Brouillet, Brouillet, and Maury (2009), which used unconstrained stems of French words. In 3,276 different participant answers, only one response achieved a degree of agreement of 60 %, and only 18 responses obtained a degree of agreement above 50 %.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In 3,276 different participant answers, only one response achieved a degree of agreement of 60 %, and only 18 responses obtained a degree of agreement above 50 %. Martin et al (2009) proposed that one way to avoid the high variability of responses and, thus, maximize priming, is to use constrained or restricted-length stems, which means limiting completions to a particular word length. According to Martin et al (2009), limiting word length avoids an overestimation of participant performance.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…E. Wilson & Horton, 2002) or approximated rates from whole-word frequency (e.g., Chiarello & Hoyer, 1988;Pope & Kern, 2006). The available databases are based on different languages (Martin et al, 2009;Olofsson & Nyberg, 1992) and, thus, are of limited value to Englishspeaking populations, or they use few stems (Graf & Williams, 1987;Lee & Leslie, 2003). Data have been collected for 914 stems using American undergraduate students (Shaw, 1997), but completions include multiple (e.g., air bag), hyphenated (e.g., cul-de-sac), and abbreviated (e.g., aren't) words.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%