2017
DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-14104-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Newly learned categories induce pre-attentive categorical perception of faces

Abstract: Face perception is modulated by categorical information in faces, which is known as categorical perception (CP) of faces. However, it remains unknown whether CP of faces is humans' inborn capability or the result of acquired categories. Here, we examined whether and when newly learned categories affect face perception. A short-term training method was employed in which participants learned new categories of face stimuli. Behaviorally, using an AB-X discrimination task, we found that the discrimination accuracy… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
14
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
1
14
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Here, the shorter vMMN latency to the oblique bar pattern (a simple stimulus) indicates that deviancy is detected earlier in the context of complex stimuli than a complex deviant event within the sequence of more simple ones. When the standard and the deviant are similar [e.g., belonging to the same category (oblique lines: Kimura et al ( 2009 , 2010 ); facial categories and emotions: Yu et al ( 2017 ), Vogel et al ( 2015 ), Kreegipuu et al ( 2013 ); left vs. right hand: Stefanics and Czigler ( 2012 )], deviant-related negativity included longer latency ranges, or there were mismatch components in various ranges, whereas in the case of highly different standard and deviant (e.g., symmetric vs. asymmetric patterns), the difference potential was confined to an earlier and narrower latency range. Furthermore, using similar standards and deviants such as disappearing parts of an object (Sulykos et al 2017 ) and checkerboards with alternating locations of the dark and light squares (Sulykos et al 2018 ), an identical earlier phase of the vMMN appeared for both younger and older groups, whereas the later part was absent or diminished in the elderly.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Here, the shorter vMMN latency to the oblique bar pattern (a simple stimulus) indicates that deviancy is detected earlier in the context of complex stimuli than a complex deviant event within the sequence of more simple ones. When the standard and the deviant are similar [e.g., belonging to the same category (oblique lines: Kimura et al ( 2009 , 2010 ); facial categories and emotions: Yu et al ( 2017 ), Vogel et al ( 2015 ), Kreegipuu et al ( 2013 ); left vs. right hand: Stefanics and Czigler ( 2012 )], deviant-related negativity included longer latency ranges, or there were mismatch components in various ranges, whereas in the case of highly different standard and deviant (e.g., symmetric vs. asymmetric patterns), the difference potential was confined to an earlier and narrower latency range. Furthermore, using similar standards and deviants such as disappearing parts of an object (Sulykos et al 2017 ) and checkerboards with alternating locations of the dark and light squares (Sulykos et al 2018 ), an identical earlier phase of the vMMN appeared for both younger and older groups, whereas the later part was absent or diminished in the elderly.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One way to control this is making the manipulated factors task-irrelevant (Firestone & Scholl, 2016). Additionally, for CP to count as an effect of language, we believe that effects should not be related to explicit perceptual training on a specific category boundary (Goldstone, Lippa, & Shiffrin, 2001;Notman, Sowden, & Özgen, 2005;Özgen & Davies, 2002;Yu et al, 2017). While perceptual training studies of CP are clearly interesting in a different context, they cannot provide evidence for a role of language in CP.…”
Section: Avoiding Experimental Confoundsmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…One recent study found color CP in the P1 component to predict access to visual consciousness in the attentional blink paradigm . Other studies found CP effects starting in the N1 time range (Boutonnet, Dering, Vinas-Guasch, & Thierry, 2013;Thierry et al, 2009;Yu et al, 2017), interpreted as a visual mismatch negativity, suggesting an effect of linguistic categories on preattentive perception (Thierry et al, 2009). In the context of visual object perception, the N1 is usually seen as an indicator of configural processing during early visual perception (Rossion & Jacques, 2011;Tanaka & Curran, 2001).…”
Section: Electrophysiological Correlates Of Categorical Perceptionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some learned CP effects are still open to the interpretation that they are not perceptual changes but a response bias from having learned to name the category (“naming bias” or “category label bias”): a tendency to judge things as less similar when their names are different and more similar when their names are the same [3739]. One way to test whether CP effects are perceptual or verbal is to analyze brain activity during category learning.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%