2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.03.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Newly-acquired words are more phonologically robust in verbal short-term memory when they have associated semantic representations

Abstract: (2016) Newlyacquired words are more phonologically robust in verbal short-term memory when they have associated semantic representations. Neuropsychologia, 98. pp. 85-97. AbstractVerbal short-term memory (STM) is a crucial cognitive function central to language learning, comprehension and reasoning, yet the processes that underlie this capacity are not fully understood. In particular, although STM primarily draws on a phonological code, interactions between long-term phonological and semantic representations… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
14
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
(99 reference statements)
0
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Consequently, all of the effects in this study could be explained in terms of the contribution of phonological-lexical representations, as opposed to a role for conceptual knowledge. Encoding tasks that require participants to attend to semantic and nonsemantic features of words have revealed fewer phoneme migrations for semantically encoded items, which aligns with the semantic-binding account (Savill, Metcalfe, Ellis, & Jefferies, 2015); however, studies investigating the effect of training new lexical-phonological forms with or without associated meanings have produced conflicting results (Benetello, Cecchetto, & Papagno, 2015;Savill et al, 2017).…”
Section: Electronic Supplementary Materialsmentioning
confidence: 72%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Consequently, all of the effects in this study could be explained in terms of the contribution of phonological-lexical representations, as opposed to a role for conceptual knowledge. Encoding tasks that require participants to attend to semantic and nonsemantic features of words have revealed fewer phoneme migrations for semantically encoded items, which aligns with the semantic-binding account (Savill, Metcalfe, Ellis, & Jefferies, 2015); however, studies investigating the effect of training new lexical-phonological forms with or without associated meanings have produced conflicting results (Benetello, Cecchetto, & Papagno, 2015;Savill et al, 2017).…”
Section: Electronic Supplementary Materialsmentioning
confidence: 72%
“…However, the relative contributions of phonological and semantic representations to verbal STM, and the ways in which they interact, are unclear. Semantic manipulations typically have relatively subtle effects on STM, as compared to phonological manipulations (e.g., Baddeley, 1966;Jefferies, Frankish, & Lambon Ralph, 2006;Majerus & van der Linden, 2003;Savill, Ellis, & Jefferies, 2017), so the consensus view is that STM is best explained by factors that influence the efficiency of phonological processing. Traditional accounts of STM suggest that the semantic contribution operates at the whole-word level-for example, activated semantic and lexical representations allow people to complete missing pieces of a phonological trace.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…At the same time, it should be noted that other evidence is not in favor of a redintegration mechanism as the exclusive account of psycholinguistic effects in vWM. For instance, strong lexicality effects have been observed in vWM tasks that do not require overt recall and redintegration (Jefferies, Frankish, & Lambon Ralph, 2006b;Kowialiewski & Majerus, 2018a;Savill, Ellis, & Jefferies, 2016). The neighborhood density effect is also of interest here.…”
Section: Consequences For Theoretical Frameworkmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…However, studies of semantic contributions to verbal short-term memory have focused on effects arising at the group level (single word repetition: Tyler et al, 2000;; immediate serial recall: Acheson, Postle, & MacDonald, 2010;Jefferies, Frankish, & Lambon Ralph, 2006;Majerus & van der Linden, 2003;Savill et al, 2018Savill et al, , 2017Savill et al, , 2015 and have not considered how these influences may vary across individuals as a function of phonological capacity, as we test here.…”
Section: (Ii)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the context of relatively preserved phonological function (Jefferies, Jones, Bateman, & Lambon Ralph, 2005), they often make specific phonological errors during the short-term recall of words that are no longer fully understood (e.g., Jefferies, Hoffman, Jones, & Lambon Ralph, 2008;Knott et al, 1997;Majerus, Norris, & Patterson, 2007; see also Peters, Majerus, De Baerdemaeker, Salmon, & Collette, 2009, for similar effects of semantic impairment in Alzheimer's Disease). Some converging support for the view that semantic information helps to stabilise the phonological trace has also been provided by studies of healthy participants (Hoffman, Jefferies, Ehsan, Jones, & Lambon Ralph, 2009;Jefferies, Frankish, & Lambon Ralph, 2006;Savill et al, 2018;Savill, Ellis, & Jefferies, 2017;Savill, Metcalfe, Ellis, & Jefferies, 2015).…”
mentioning
confidence: 91%