2015
DOI: 10.1002/dys.1515
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Neurocognitive Development and Predictors of L1 and L2 Literacy Skills in Dyslexia: A Longitudinal Study of Children 5–11 Years Old

Abstract: The aim of this study was to find valid neurocognitive precursors of literacy development in first language (L1, Norwegian) and second language (L2, English) in a group of children during their Pre‐literacy, Emergent Literacy and Literacy stages, by comparing children with dyslexia and a typical group. Children who were 5 years old at project start were followed until the age of 11, when dyslexia was identified and data could be analysed in retrospect.The children's neurocognitive pattern changed both by liter… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
34
0
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(37 citation statements)
references
References 78 publications
2
34
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In the last 40 years many studies have investigated the relationship between RAN and reading in children with different reading abilities and found that RAN is an early and significant predictor of reading across different orthographies. RAN is related to reading abilities Helland & Morken, 2016;Papadopoulos, Spanoudis, & Georgiou, 2016;Peterson et al, 2018;Zoccolotti, De Luca, & Spinelli, 2015) regardless of the type of stimulus to be named (Landerl et al, 2013;Papadopoulos et al, 2016;van den Bos, Zijlstra, & Spelberg, 2002).…”
Section: Practitioner Pointsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In the last 40 years many studies have investigated the relationship between RAN and reading in children with different reading abilities and found that RAN is an early and significant predictor of reading across different orthographies. RAN is related to reading abilities Helland & Morken, 2016;Papadopoulos, Spanoudis, & Georgiou, 2016;Peterson et al, 2018;Zoccolotti, De Luca, & Spinelli, 2015) regardless of the type of stimulus to be named (Landerl et al, 2013;Papadopoulos et al, 2016;van den Bos, Zijlstra, & Spelberg, 2002).…”
Section: Practitioner Pointsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the last 40 years many studies have investigated the relationship between RAN and reading in children with different reading abilities and found that RAN is an early and significant predictor of reading across different orthographies. RAN is related to reading abilities (Di Filippo et al, , ; Helland & Morken, ; Papadopoulos, Spanoudis, & Georgiou, ; Peterson et al, ; Zoccolotti, De Luca, & Spinelli, ) regardless of the type of stimulus to be named (Landerl et al, ; Papadopoulos et al, ; van den Bos, Zijlstra, & Spelberg, ). Some studies and meta‐analyses suggest, however, that alphanumeric RAN stimuli are more strongly related to reading than are nonalphanumeric stimuli (Araújo, Reis, Petersson, & Faísca, ; Georgiou, Aro, Liao, & Parrila, ; Song, Georgiou, Su, & Hua, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…At the cognitive level, the same group of children showed a variety of deficits with language processing, visual and auditory memory functions, attention, and executive functions, but degree and impact of the deficits changed with age. However, early scores of RAN and digit span seemed to be reliable predictors of later reading and spelling outcomes (Helland & Morken, ). Other studies have found that children's early RAN skills and literacy skills appear to have a reciprocal effect (Peterson et al, ; Wolff, ).…”
Section: The Reading Network and Literacy Skills In Dyslexiamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…L1 phonological awareness, especially syllable and phoneme awareness, predicted EFL decoding abilities in 5–6-year old Korean children, who were instructed in English (Kang, 2012). Similarly, L1 phonological awareness of 5–6-year old pre-literacy, Year 1 pre-school Norwegian students predicted their subsequent EFL spelling, word reading, and translation skills when 11-year old (Helland and Morken, 2016). Thus, a cross-linguistic transfer of phonological skills occurs in educational contexts of different FL exposure.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Syllable and intrasyllabic element awareness precedes letter identification (Awramiuk and Krasowicz-Kupis, 2014), while phonological sensitivity and letter knowledge reciprocally contribute to the development of one another prior to formal reading instruction (Burgess and Lonigan, 1998). In 5-year old Chinese children, syllable awareness predicted word reading at ages 8 and 10 (Pan et al, 2011), and in 5–6-year old Norwegian native speakers phonological awareness predicted spelling, word reading, and translation at age 11 (Helland and Morken, 2016) in learning EFL. In a bit older 6-year old English native speakers, at an early literacy stage, reading readiness (as measured with, e.g., phonological awareness tasks: rhyming and letter-sound relationships) predicted L2 (Spanish, French, and German) proficiency in Year 10 (Sparks et al, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%