Our system is currently under heavy load due to increased usage. We're actively working on upgrades to improve performance. Thank you for your patience.
2006
DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.09.088
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Neural substrates of sarcasm: A functional magnetic-resonance imaging study

Abstract: The understanding of sarcasm reflects a complex process, which involves recognizing the beliefs of the speaker. There is a clear association between deficits in mentalizing, which is the ability to understand other people's behavior in terms of their mental state, and the understanding of sarcasm in individuals with autistic spectrum disorders. This suggests that mentalizing is important in pragmatic non-literal language comprehension. To highlight the neural substrates of sarcasm, 20 normal adult volunteers u… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

5
57
0
3

Year Published

2012
2012
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 148 publications
(65 citation statements)
references
References 68 publications
5
57
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…One is that the vignettes and their target utterances are almost invariably short, ranging from two to at most four sentences (Eviatar and Just, 2006;Rapp et al, 2010;Shibata et al, 2010;Uchiyama et al, 2006Uchiyama et al, , 2011Wakusawa et al, 2007;Wang et al, 2006). Arguably, such brevity gives a participant a limited amount of time to appreciate the background of a story and its eventual irony (as in 2a above).…”
Section: Processing Ironymentioning
confidence: 96%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…One is that the vignettes and their target utterances are almost invariably short, ranging from two to at most four sentences (Eviatar and Just, 2006;Rapp et al, 2010;Shibata et al, 2010;Uchiyama et al, 2006Uchiyama et al, , 2011Wakusawa et al, 2007;Wang et al, 2006). Arguably, such brevity gives a participant a limited amount of time to appreciate the background of a story and its eventual irony (as in 2a above).…”
Section: Processing Ironymentioning
confidence: 96%
“…In two studies (e.g., Shibata et al, 2010;Wakusawa et al, 2007), ironic versus literal stories are not designed from common contexts. In three other studies (Eviatar and Just, 2006;Uchiyama et al, 2006;Wang et al, 2006), as Rapp et al (2010) point out, Ironic and Literal conditions are not directly compared. For example, Uchiyama et al's (2006) sarcasm-detection measure was determined by contrasting, on the one hand, the sarcastic and the non-sarcastic remarks together and, on the other, a control sentence that was "unconnected."…”
Section: Processing Ironymentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The strongest and largest cluster was found in the left inferior frontal gyrus in regions equivalent to Brodmann area 13/45 with extension into the middle frontal gyrus (Brodmann area 9). The second strongest cluster was located in the left middle/superior temporal gyrus (Brodmann area 22) with contributions from studies on irony (Shibata et al, 2010;Uchiyama et al, 2006), metaphor (Ahrens et al, 2007;, idioms (Boulenger et al, 2009Zempleni et al, 2007), and metonymy .…”
Section: Meta-analysis 1: All Non-literal Versus Literal Stimulimentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Subsequently, more than 15 additional fMRI metaphor studies followed (Table 1). The first fMRI studies on ironic expressions were published in 2006 (Eviatar and Just, 2006;Uchiyama et al, 2006;Wang et al, 2006a), followed by work on idioms (Zempleni, 2006) and metonymy . In their first study, Rapp and colleagues determined that the anterior-inferior part of the left IFG in particular plays a key role in disentangling non-literal meanings Rapp et al, 2004).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%