2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.03.022
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Neural evidence that inhibition is linked to the affective devaluation of distractors that match the contents of working memory

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

1
18
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

4
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
1
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Many prior studies of distractor devaluation have included a novel baseline condition in which ratings were obtained for previously-unseen items with no history of being either attended or ignored in prior trials (De Vito, Al-Aidroos, & Fenske, 2017;Duff & Faber, 2011;Goolsby, Shapiro, Silvert, et al, 2009;Griffiths & Mitchell, 2008;Kihara, Yagi, Takeda, & Kawahara, 2011;Martiny-Huenger et al, 2014;Raymond et al, 2003;Veling et al, 2007). The typical finding from these studies is that distractors are rated more negatively than novel-baseline items, while ratings of targets do not differ from those of novelbaseline items (i.e., distractor devaluation, not target valuation).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Many prior studies of distractor devaluation have included a novel baseline condition in which ratings were obtained for previously-unseen items with no history of being either attended or ignored in prior trials (De Vito, Al-Aidroos, & Fenske, 2017;Duff & Faber, 2011;Goolsby, Shapiro, Silvert, et al, 2009;Griffiths & Mitchell, 2008;Kihara, Yagi, Takeda, & Kawahara, 2011;Martiny-Huenger et al, 2014;Raymond et al, 2003;Veling et al, 2007). The typical finding from these studies is that distractors are rated more negatively than novel-baseline items, while ratings of targets do not differ from those of novelbaseline items (i.e., distractor devaluation, not target valuation).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An individual is less likely to expend effort seeking a motivationally relevant stimulus, not because the ability to execute the necessary motor actions has been affected but because the stimulus itself has become less motivationally appealing. Several recent findings support this possible link between inhibition and stimulus devaluation, including the consistent observations that stimuli that are ignored or otherwise inhibited in selective attention (e.g., Raymond, Fenske, & Tavassoli, 2003;De Vito, Al-Aidroos, & Fenske, 2017;Fenske, Raymond, & Kunar, 2004;Fenske, Raymond, Kessler, Westoby, & Tipper, 2005;Kiss et al, 2007; or memory-suppression tasks Vivas, Marful, Panagiotidou, & Bajo, 2016), or from which a response is withheld in go/no-go (e.g., Frischen, Ferrey, Burt, Pistchick, & Fenske, 2012;Ferrey et al, 2012;Kiss, Raymond, Westoby, Nobre, & Eimer, 2008) or stop-signal tasks (Wessel, Tonnesen, & Aron, 2015) subsequently receive more negative affective evaluations than the targets of attention/response. Such cognitive-behavioral and neuroimaging results have been taken as evidence that inhibition itself may alter the coding and representation of stimulus value (for reviews, see Fenske & Raymond, 2006;Raymond, 2009;.…”
mentioning
confidence: 82%
“…If the level of inhibition indicated by relatively slowed response times on ABA trials is indeed linked to the affective devaluation of the associated Mondrian images, then fluctuations in the magnitude of response-time slowing should be linked to the magnitude of stimulus devaluation. We assessed this using a modified version of the approach developed by Kiss et al (2007Kiss et al ( , 2008; see also Clancy, Fiacconi & Fenske, 2019;De Vito, Al-Aidroos & Fenske, 2017;Doallo et al, 2012) for investigating how neural and electrophysiological activity occurring during a task involving stimulus/response-inhibition can be predictive of subsequent affective ratings of the stimuli in that task. To examine whether fluctuations in the main behavioural index of backward inhibition-slowed response times-were linked to the magnitude of affective devaluation of ABA items, we first sorted all of the ABA sequences into two categories for each participant based on whether they gave the final-trial image from each sequence a relatively negative rating (i.e., Low-rating: bottom 40% of trials with lowest ratings) or a relatively positive rating (i.e., High-rating: top 40% of trials with highest ratings).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is a wide range of cognitive domains in which inhibition-based processes are thought to be critical for successful performance, and evidence of a link between inhibition and negative stimulus-linked affect has been found in several of them. This includes devaluation of stimuli associated with behavioural inhibition in Go/No-go tasks (Fenske et al, 2005;Kiss et al, 2008;Veling, Holland, & Van Knipperberg, 2008) and Stop-signal tasks (Wessel & O'Doherty, 2014;Wessel, Tonnesen, & Aron, 2015), with attentional inhibition in visual search (Fenske et al, 2004;Raymond et al, 2005), flanker (Martiny-Huenger, , attentional blink (Kihara, Yagi, Takeda, & Kawahara, 2011) and distractor-avoidance tasks (De Vito, Al-Aidroos, & Fenske, 2017), with long-term memory suppression in Think/No-think (De Vito and and Directed-forgetting tasks (Vivas, Marful, Panagiotidou, & Bajo, 2016;cf. Janczyk & Wühr, 2012) and with the inhibition of working memory representations in retro-cue memory-search tasks (De Vito & Fenske, 2018;De Vito, Ferrey, Fenske, & Al-Aidroos, 2018).…”
Section: Stimulus Devaluation By Backward Inhibition Exceeds Any Emotmentioning
confidence: 99%