2005
DOI: 10.1016/j.acra.2004.11.023
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Neural correlates of telling lies

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

4
24
0
2

Year Published

2007
2007
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 150 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
4
24
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The anterior cingulate activation that was found by Langleben et al (2002) and Kozel et al (2005) was consistent with our findings. The MPFC activations that were found by Ganis et al (2003), Phan et al (2005), and Abe et al (2008) were also consistent with many self-relevance studies, as described in the introduction. However, the activation in the right superior frontal gyrus that was reported by Abe et al (2008) is not consistent with the findings of self-relevance studies and with our findings.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 82%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The anterior cingulate activation that was found by Langleben et al (2002) and Kozel et al (2005) was consistent with our findings. The MPFC activations that were found by Ganis et al (2003), Phan et al (2005), and Abe et al (2008) were also consistent with many self-relevance studies, as described in the introduction. However, the activation in the right superior frontal gyrus that was reported by Abe et al (2008) is not consistent with the findings of self-relevance studies and with our findings.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 82%
“…Unlike lie detection, whether the subject tells the truth or a lie is not our concern. In many fMRI studies (Abe et al, 2008;Ganis, Kosslyn, Stose, Thompson, & Yurgelun-Todd, 2003;Kozel et al, 2005;Langleben et al, 2002;Phan et al, 2005), brain activity comparisons between deceptive and truthful responses have been found to be different. The anterior cingulate activation that was found by Langleben et al (2002) and Kozel et al (2005) was consistent with our findings.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…One possibility is that the intention to conceal elicits a deception-specific process during the CIT, and ERPs reflect it. The existence of such a deception-specific process has been demonstrated in ERP studies (e.g., Johnson et al 2003Johnson et al , 2004Johnson et al , 2005 and in brain imaging studies (e.g., Abe et al 2006;Ganis et al 2003;Langleben et al 2002;Phan et al 2005). The other possibility is that a general process not specific to deception, such as need for additional processing or increased significance/salience of an item, is responsible for the P300 increase.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…Although the CIT is not a test of deception, it was reasoned that the denial of relevant-item knowledge causes a conflict between the truthful and the required deceptive response, which is resolved by inhibiting the former response (Seymour & Schumacher, 2009;Verschuere & De Houwer, 2011). Response inhibition has been indicated to play a role in the CIT based on Reaction Time (Suchotzki, Verschuere, Peth, Crombez, & Gamer, 2014) and fMRI measures (e.g., Gamer, 2014;Gamer, Bauermann, Stoeter, & Vossel, 2007;Langleben et al, 2002;Phan et al, 2005), but it remains unclear whether it plays a role in the autonomic-based CIT (see Ambach, Stark, Peper, & Vaitl, 2008a;Ambach et al, 2011). Indeed, meta-analytic studies have demonstrated that similar CIT effects were observed with the SCR measure when subjects responded deceptively to the relevant items and when they remained silent and did not give any overt responses (Ben-Shakhar & Elaad, 2003;Meijer, klein Selle, Elber, & Ben-Shakhar, 2014).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%