2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.07.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Neural correlates of metonymy resolution

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
14
2

Year Published

2012
2012
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 27 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
2
14
2
Order By: Relevance
“…No other comparisons at this ROI were significant. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that indeterminate sentences might engage greater RH resources than preferred sentences, and also consistent with studies that show R-IFG activation for metonymic sentences (e.g., Africa is hungry , Rapp et al, 2011), which arguably require pragmatic processes for recovering the proper interpretation of the subject noun. However, indeterminate sentences did not differ from non-preferred and full-VP sentences, suggesting that as constructions deviate from the canonical form, they show attenuated margins of differentiation with the indeterminate condition.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 90%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…No other comparisons at this ROI were significant. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that indeterminate sentences might engage greater RH resources than preferred sentences, and also consistent with studies that show R-IFG activation for metonymic sentences (e.g., Africa is hungry , Rapp et al, 2011), which arguably require pragmatic processes for recovering the proper interpretation of the subject noun. However, indeterminate sentences did not differ from non-preferred and full-VP sentences, suggesting that as constructions deviate from the canonical form, they show attenuated margins of differentiation with the indeterminate condition.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Bambini et al (2011) also found greater involvement of right superior temporal sulcus (STS) and middle FG in metaphor processing, suggesting that quintessential pragmatic processes engage different RH neural substrates—or engage them to a larger extent—when compared to literal language (see also Bottini et al, 1994; Kacinik and Chiarello, 2007) 3 . We see a similar pattern even in studies showing LH-dominance in the comprehension of metonymy (Rapp et al, 2011): the R-IFG shows significant activation for metonymic ( Hitchcock is worth watching ) expressions beyond what is required to activate literal expressions ( Hitchcock is dead ). Other forms of figurative language processing—such as irony and sarcasm—also suggest the activation of bilateral networks, with significant RH clusters compared to literal controls (for a review see Bohrn et al, 2012).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…A comparison of (6c) versus (6d) provides a test of the hypothesis that metonymic processing depends on sentence structure. Support for this hypothesis would help reconcile conflicting results on whether there is a processing cost associated with interpretation of metonyms (Frisson & Pickering, 1999, 2007; Ghio et al, 2012; Gibbs, 1990; Humphrey et al, 2004; McElree et al, 2006; Rapp et al, 2011; Weiland et al, 2012). In addition Experiment 2 tested whether interpreting familiar metonyms imposes a processing cost when compared to a different baseline.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…This said, Rapp et al (2011) do report on decontextualized metonymy comprehension, which we take as an initial guide to our localization predictions. Rapp et al (2011) present metonymies such as "Africa is hungry," in which a causal link (supplied by historical information expected to be known by the comprehender) must be created to connect the geographic designation of Africa with its inhabitants.…”
Section: Previous Work On Metonymymentioning
confidence: 98%