2019
DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2019.09.007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Neighboring chromaticity influences how white a surface looks

Abstract: To identify surface properties independently of the illumination the visual system must make assumptions about the statistics of scenes and their illumination. Are assumptions about the intensity of the illumination independent of assumptions about its chromaticity? To find out, we asked participants to judge whether test patches within three different sets of surrounding surfaces were white or grey. Two sets were matched in terms of their maximal luminance, their mean luminance and chromaticity, and the varia… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
1
1

Relationship

0
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It has also been found that the lightness of highly saturated chromatic colours influences the perceived chromaticity of the white-point [26,27], an observation potentially in line with the idea of chromatic anchoring. Still another study found that perceived lightness of the brightest colours was lower when the probe stimulus was surrounded by more saturated green, blue, and red colours [28]. This observation is in line with chromatic anchoring of lightness because higher saturation of green, blue, and red implies that the white-point is higher in brightness and hence the lightness scale involves higher levels of brightness.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 67%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…It has also been found that the lightness of highly saturated chromatic colours influences the perceived chromaticity of the white-point [26,27], an observation potentially in line with the idea of chromatic anchoring. Still another study found that perceived lightness of the brightest colours was lower when the probe stimulus was surrounded by more saturated green, blue, and red colours [28]. This observation is in line with chromatic anchoring of lightness because higher saturation of green, blue, and red implies that the white-point is higher in brightness and hence the lightness scale involves higher levels of brightness.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 67%
“…Despite the low resolution of the colour naming measure and the minimalism of our set-up we obtained clear evidence for chromatic anchoring: While displays without chromatic anchor were almost unanimously judged white (Figure 2.a-d), those with the red and two of the blue chromatic anchors (Figure 2.e-j) were almost unanimously judged grey. These pronounced effects of chromatic anchors contrast the subtle effects of saturation on lightness judgements observed in a previous study that used a much richer, patterned display with many colours [28]. That study also used repeated measurements for each observer, which should lead to a higher signal to noise ratio compared to our single measurement per participant.…”
Section: Evidencementioning
confidence: 87%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Such an effect may be described as a uniform veil that acted in a spatially uniform manner on the image 31,32 . The model in use here was applied solely to spectral stimulus that reached the eye of the observer and did not independently consider the effect of atmospheric variations as a veil, how they could be partially discounted 33 or how the surrounding colors could impact local perception of the image 34 . Observers could use saturation as a cue to perform the task in the psychophysical experiment, but as negative‐distance images become more saturated, this cue would not provide indication of the original image.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%