2018
DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12246
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Negativity bias in infants’ expectations about agents’ dispositions

Abstract: This study investigated 6- and 10-month-old infants' abilities to infer others' preferences based on social interactions using looking time and choice measures. Infants were randomly assigned to either a helping/neutral or hindering/neutral condition. Those in the helping/neutral condition were first familiarized with a helping event, in which an agent helped a circle climb a hill, and a neutral event, in which another agent followed the same path as the helping agent but had no interaction with the circle. Du… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
19
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 57 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
(74 reference statements)
2
19
1
Order By: Relevance
“…These findings revealed that 10‐month‐olds are capable to understand the intention behind intervening to prevent someone being harmed. These results are in line with previous findings, suggesting that infants can infer or predict an agent's disposition toward another agent, based on whether a previous social interaction between the agents was positive or negative (Chae & Song, 2018; Fawcett & Liszkowski, 2012; Hamlin et al, 2007; Kuhlmeier, Wynn, & Bloom, 2003; Lee, Yun, Kim, & Song, 2015). Taken together, these previous findings suggest that infants can evaluate prosocial or antisocial actions and can generate their own preferences or infer others’ preference for positive agents.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…These findings revealed that 10‐month‐olds are capable to understand the intention behind intervening to prevent someone being harmed. These results are in line with previous findings, suggesting that infants can infer or predict an agent's disposition toward another agent, based on whether a previous social interaction between the agents was positive or negative (Chae & Song, 2018; Fawcett & Liszkowski, 2012; Hamlin et al, 2007; Kuhlmeier, Wynn, & Bloom, 2003; Lee, Yun, Kim, & Song, 2015). Taken together, these previous findings suggest that infants can evaluate prosocial or antisocial actions and can generate their own preferences or infer others’ preference for positive agents.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…A number of studies have tried to replicate and extend the findings of Hamlin and colleagues [ 7 , 8 , 10 ]. The results were often in line with the original findings (e.g., [ 13 24 ]) with seven studies not conducted by Hamlin and colleagues [ 13 , 14 , 19 22 ]. This overall impression was confirmed by a recent meta-analysis that focused on young children’s manual choice between a prosocial and an antisocial agent [ 11 ].…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 89%
“…The current study included a total of 44 infant/parent dyads (20 in Experiment 1 and 24 in Experiment 2), and these numbers were determined prior to the start of data collection. Although recommended (Simmons et al, 2011), a description for pre-determining sample size is rarely provided in published studies using the single-choice infant paradigm (e.g., Hamlin et al, 2007, 2010, 2013; Mahajan and Wynn, 2012; Gerson et al, 2017; Chae and Song, 2018; Margoni and Surian, 2018) and at times is not included as part of the study protocol (Eason et al, 2017). Small sample sizes are problematic, as they increase the likelihood of spurious findings (Oakes, 2017).…”
Section: Limitationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Infants as young as 5 months (Hamlin et al, 2007, 2010, 2011; Hamlin and Wynn, 2011, 2012) and older infants and toddlers (Geraci and Surian, 2011; Buon et al, 2014; Scola et al, 2015; Woo et al, 2017; Chae and Song, 2018) seem capable of socially evaluating the behavior of others and appear to show a preference for prosocial others (for reviews, see Martin and Olson, 2015; Holvoet et al, 2016; Van de Vondervoort and Hamlin, 2018) as well as those who are similar to themselves along some dimension (e.g., Hamlin and Wynn, 2012; Mahajan and Wynn, 2012; Hamlin et al, 2013; Burns and Sommerville, 2014; Gerson et al, 2017). These findings have led researchers to hypothesize that we may be born with something akin to an innate moral core (Cook, 2013; Hamlin, 2013) or early strong tendencies (Martin and Olson, 2015; Holvoet et al, 2016; Hare, 2017), which include a preference for similar others (Hamlin and Wynn, 2012; Mahajan and Wynn, 2012; Hamlin et al, 2013) and that these tendencies observed during infancy may predict social and behavioral adjustment at 4 years of age (Tan et al, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%