2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.08.025
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Negative mood, implicit alcohol-related memory, and alcohol use in young adults: The moderating effect of alcohol expectancy

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

1
11
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
1
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The relationship between subjective reactivity and mood-induced alcohol-seeking supports prior weak evidence for this relationship from three alcohol studies ( Kelly et al, 2011 ; Owens et al, 2015 ; Sinha et al, 2009 ), and strong evidence from one smoking study ( Hogarth et al, 2015 ), and contradicts two null findings ( Magrys & Olmstead, 2015 ; McGrath et al, 2016 ). Despite subjective reactivity being associated with mood-induced alcohol choice, there is a question as to whether this measure is a clinically meaningful marker, because it did not correlate positively with depression symptoms, coping motives, or alcohol use disorder severity.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 57%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The relationship between subjective reactivity and mood-induced alcohol-seeking supports prior weak evidence for this relationship from three alcohol studies ( Kelly et al, 2011 ; Owens et al, 2015 ; Sinha et al, 2009 ), and strong evidence from one smoking study ( Hogarth et al, 2015 ), and contradicts two null findings ( Magrys & Olmstead, 2015 ; McGrath et al, 2016 ). Despite subjective reactivity being associated with mood-induced alcohol choice, there is a question as to whether this measure is a clinically meaningful marker, because it did not correlate positively with depression symptoms, coping motives, or alcohol use disorder severity.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 57%
“…There is also mixed evidence as to whether mood-induced alcohol-seeking is associated with subjective emotional reactivity to negative triggers. In relation to this association, there is weak evidence from three alcohol studies ( Kelly, Masterman, & Young, 2011 ; Owens et al, 2015 ; Sinha et al, 2009 ), strong evidence from one smoking study ( Hogarth et al, 2015 ), and nonsignificant correlations reported in two studies ( Magrys & Olmstead, 2015 ; McGrath et al, 2016 ). Therefore, the current study evaluated the association between mood-induced alcohol-seeking and subjective mood reactivity, to address this mixed literature and the possible role of mood-regulation skills in alcohol dependence ( Berking et al, 2011 ).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, people characterized by this kind of style overall believe that their well-being is under their responsibility, under their direct control (Health Locus of Control). Some studies in literature show that people who exert a control on their health have a positive thinking toward their health status and are able to modulate emotional distress caused by a disease, trying to face it actively with protective behaviors [94,95]. For example, Kidd and colleagues [96] reported that patients with advanced coronary heart disease who had high levels of personal health control showed better outcomes in emotional reactions (e.g., depression reduction), physical activity, and quality of life after 3 months post-surgery (coronary artery bypass graft).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This approach suggests the importance in clinical practice of promoting positive emotions in patients. In the health domain, research suggests that global affective statesfeeling good or bad contribute to unhealthy behaviors such as smoking [94,[99][100][101], alcohol consumption [95,102], and overeating [103]. Emotions also contribute to health-related risk perceptions [104], for example, worry about a health threat may trigger preventive behaviors [105].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The first group found that: a) negative affective cues eased the activation of IAC in coping motives and problem drinkers and this activation predicted alcohol consumption (Campos-Melady & Smith, 2012;Zack, Toneatto & MacLeod, 1999); b) high doses of benzodiazepines reduced that activation while low doses eased it (Zack, Poulos & Woodford, 2006); c) there was a strong relationship between depressive symptoms and positive implicit alcohol associations in coping motives drinkers (Ralston & Palfai, 2012); and d) coping motives drinkers present higher alcohol attentional bias than no coping motives drinkers (Forestell, Dickter &Young, 2012). The second group of articles found that: a) an induction of a negative emotional state predicted IAC in high risk drinkers (Kelly & Masterman, 2008), in CM drinkers (Ostafin & Brooks, 2011), in individuals with high tendency to act impulsively during negative emotional states (Treloar & McCarthy, 2012) and in individuals with weak implicit expectancies that alcohol generates negative emotions (Kelly, Masterman & Young, 2011); b) an induction of an anxiety emotional state predicted IAC in coping-anxiety motive drinkers; and c) an induction of a positive emotional state predicted IAC in low risk drinkers (Kelly & Masterman, 2008) and enhancement motive drinkers (Birch, Stewart, Wiers, Klein, MacLean & Berish, 2008;Grant, Stewart & Birch, 2007).…”
Section: Topic 6: Context Influence On Iacmentioning
confidence: 97%