2016
DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12234
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Narrative abilities of monolingual and bilingual children with and without language impairment: implications for clinical practice

Abstract: This study confirms the hypothesis that measures of narrative macro-structure are not biased against children who have less experience with a particular language, like bilinguals. In addition, it indicates that using narratives to assess children's language abilities can support the identification of LI in both a monolingual and a bilingual context.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

7
83
1
16

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 85 publications
(107 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
(91 reference statements)
7
83
1
16
Order By: Relevance
“…However, despite these difficulties, they were able to retell a short, well-sequenced, and accurate story. We therefore reiterate our opinion that a text-level task such as the PONA is a more ecologically valid way of appraising oral language performance in young school-age students [43]. …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, despite these difficulties, they were able to retell a short, well-sequenced, and accurate story. We therefore reiterate our opinion that a text-level task such as the PONA is a more ecologically valid way of appraising oral language performance in young school-age students [43]. …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Informal measures reduce the bias of standardised norm-referenced assessments that over-identify students from culturally diverse or low socioeconomic backgrounds [40, 41]. For example, there is a growing body of evidence supporting the use of oral narrative tasks to appraise student language abilities in both monolingual and bilingual students [42, 43]. The findings from these studies indicate student performance on oral narrative tasks was similar for both groups of students, suggesting bilingual students were not disadvantaged by this measure [42, 43].…”
Section: Assessment Of Oral Language Proficiencymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To be specific, TD bilingual children do not differ from monolingual peers in narrative macrostructure, which concerns the inclusion of key story elements within the narrative (Boerma et al, 2016;Hipfner-Boucher et al, 2014;Rezzonico et al, 2015;Rodina, 2016). Moreover, monolingual and bilingual children with language impairment show comparably impaired narrative macrostructure (Boerma et al, 2016;Cleave et al, 2010;Rezzonico et al, 2015). Other studies have found that bilingual children with language impairment show poorer narrative macrostructure than TD bilingual peers (Paradis et al, 2013;Squires et al, 2014), though there are notable exceptions (Iluz-Cohen & Walters, 2012;Tsimpli et al, 2016).…”
mentioning
confidence: 92%
“…Moreover, measures of expressive vocabulary and receptive grammar also did not discriminate between these groups at age 6 years. Furthermore, Boerma, Leseman, Timmermeister, Wijnen, and Blom (2016) and Thordardottir and Brandeker (2013) found that although TD bilingual children outperformed bilingual peers with language impairment on a measure of receptive vocabulary, they performed comparably to monolingual children with language impairment.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation