2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.016
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Naming of short words is (almost) the same as naming of alphanumeric symbols: Evidence from two orthographies

Abstract: Throughout reading development, a gradual shift is seen in the processes underlying word identification from serial decoding toward parallel processing or sight word reading. It has been argued that this shift can be detected in the correlations between serial and discrete naming of alphanumeric symbols (digits and letters) and words. In the current study, we examined the relations between alphanumeric symbol naming and reading of monosyllabic and multisyllabic words and nonwords in two languages that differ i… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
36
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

5
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(37 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
1
36
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We have argued elsewhere (e.g., Bakos, Landerl, Bartling, Schulte-Körne, & Moll, 2018;Moll & Landerl, 2009), that children with RD store and use orthographic representations during reading, but are slow in processing their phonological form. Thus, reading fluency deficits may result from slow visual-verbal access (Lervåg & Hulme, 2009) and inefficient sequential processing (Jones, Obregón, Kelly, & Branigan, 2008;Protopapas, Katopodi, Altani, & Georgiou, 2018;van Den Boer, Georgiou, & de Jong, 2016). This interpretation is supported by strong associations between (deficits in) reading fluency and RAN (Araújo & Faísca, 2019;Araújo, Reis, Petersson, & Faísca, 2015;Kirby, Georgiou, Martinussen, & Parrila, 2010).…”
Section: Reading Fluency Deficitsmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…We have argued elsewhere (e.g., Bakos, Landerl, Bartling, Schulte-Körne, & Moll, 2018;Moll & Landerl, 2009), that children with RD store and use orthographic representations during reading, but are slow in processing their phonological form. Thus, reading fluency deficits may result from slow visual-verbal access (Lervåg & Hulme, 2009) and inefficient sequential processing (Jones, Obregón, Kelly, & Branigan, 2008;Protopapas, Katopodi, Altani, & Georgiou, 2018;van Den Boer, Georgiou, & de Jong, 2016). This interpretation is supported by strong associations between (deficits in) reading fluency and RAN (Araújo & Faísca, 2019;Araújo, Reis, Petersson, & Faísca, 2015;Kirby, Georgiou, Martinussen, & Parrila, 2010).…”
Section: Reading Fluency Deficitsmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…The serial format produces shorter total naming times than discrete naming, that is, a serial advantage is observed, at least for typical readers (Zoccolotti et al, 2013). In addition, it has long been noted that reading fluency correlates more strongly with serial naming than with discrete naming (Wolf & Bowers, 1999); this serial superiority effect has been recently confirmed across languages, for readers past the beginner stage (e.g., de Jong, 2011;Protopapas et al, 2013a;van den Boer, Georgiou, & de Jong, 2016). The crucial distinction between serial and discrete naming is thought to concern sequential processing, that is, skill in the serial processing of successive stimuli.…”
Section: Differential Associations With Serial and Discrete Namingmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Second, all items in Greek consisted of two-syllable words, while items in English consisted of one-syllable words. Strictly matching items across and within these two languages was impossible, as most single-digit number words are bisyllabic in Greek, but monosyllabic in English (see Georgiou et al, 2016, for previous studies mentioning the same problem). Still, the great similarity in the pattern of results obtained across the two orthographies somewhat alleviates the concern that such differences in materials may have introduced consequential confounds.…”
Section: Limitationsmentioning
confidence: 99%