1968
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1968.11-517
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

MULTIPLE SCHEDULES: EFFECTS OF THE DISTRIBUTION OF REINFORCEMENTS BETWEEN COMPONENTS ON THE DISTRIBUTION OF RESPONSES BETWEEN COMPONENTS1

Abstract: Two pigeons were trained to peck a key under several multiple variable-interval variableinterval schedules of reinforcement; different numbers of reinforcements were scheduled in two components of equal duration which were correlated with red and green illumination of the response key respectively. The results showed: (1) that the total number of responses in a scssion was proportional to the one-sixth power of the total number of reinforcements delivered in that session; and (2) that the ratio of responses be… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

17
105
2
2

Year Published

1971
1971
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 86 publications
(126 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
17
105
2
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Starting nearest the lower left corner, the points represent the following relative response and reinforcement rates: VI 60 sec/(VI 60 sec + VI 15 sec); VI 60 sec/(VI60 sec + VI 120 sec); VIIS sec/ (VIIS sec + VI 120 sec). Schedule relative response rates showed the expected tendency toward undermatching (Lander & Irwin, 1968;Reynolds, 1963), a result consistent with the reverse-S-shaped curve expected from Herrnstein's (1970) Average choice probe relative response as a function of training relative reinforcement rate is presented in Figure 2. The order of the points corresponds to that in Figure 1.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 78%
“…Starting nearest the lower left corner, the points represent the following relative response and reinforcement rates: VI 60 sec/(VI 60 sec + VI 15 sec); VI 60 sec/(VI60 sec + VI 120 sec); VIIS sec/ (VIIS sec + VI 120 sec). Schedule relative response rates showed the expected tendency toward undermatching (Lander & Irwin, 1968;Reynolds, 1963), a result consistent with the reverse-S-shaped curve expected from Herrnstein's (1970) Average choice probe relative response as a function of training relative reinforcement rate is presented in Figure 2. The order of the points corresponds to that in Figure 1.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 78%
“…in the unaltered (no-loss) components. Such interactions between responding across components are common in multiple schedules (e.g., Catania, 1961;Lander & Irwin, 1968;Reynolds, 1961), including those involving punishment (Azrin & Holz, 1966). Prior research has shown, for example, that unpunished responding increases as a function of punishment contingencies arranged in a second component (Brethower & Reynolds, 1962;Caplan & Graefe, 1980;Crosbie, Williams, Lattal, Anderson, & Brown, 1997;Lattal & Griffin, 1972), a phenomenon termed punishment contrast.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As noted above, even responding on simple fixed-T response-initiated delay (RID) schedules violates maximization. Matching is also highly overdetermined, in the sense that almost any learning rule consistent with the law of effect-an increase in reinforcement probability causes an increase in response probability-will yield either simple matching (Equation 5) or its power-law generalization (Baum 1974, Hinson & Staddon 1983, Lander & Irwin 1968, Staddon 1968. Matching by itself therefore reveals relatively little about the dynamic processes operating in the responding subject (but see Davison & Baum 2000).…”
Section: Concurrent Schedulesmentioning
confidence: 99%