2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.031
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Multi-modal distraction: Insights from children’s limited attention

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
79
4
2

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 75 publications
(96 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
9
79
4
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Matusz et al (2015) asked 6-year-olds, 11-year-olds and adults to search for a coloured shape according to a conjunction of features (a red square or a green circle) amongst other non-target shapes (red and green triangles, circles, and squares). The perceptual load of the task was manipulated by either presenting the target shape alone (low perceptual load), or together with three non-target shapes (high perceptual load) in one of six possible locations.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Matusz et al (2015) asked 6-year-olds, 11-year-olds and adults to search for a coloured shape according to a conjunction of features (a red square or a green circle) amongst other non-target shapes (red and green triangles, circles, and squares). The perceptual load of the task was manipulated by either presenting the target shape alone (low perceptual load), or together with three non-target shapes (high perceptual load) in one of six possible locations.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Generally, it may be more valid to instead specify the conditions under which a given neural event may be elicited or not elicited, rather than to classify it into semi-binary categories. Furthermore, our results open an exciting possibility that automaticity of brain/cognitive processes and its dependence on experience (Astle and Scerif, 2011;Amso and Scerif, 2015;Murray et al, 2015b) can perhaps be more accurately understood by testing them in settings that resemble more naturalistic environments (e.g., Matusz et al, 2015b). In such environments, there is a variety of dimensions along which the stimuli that are present at any point time differ.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…More recently, the context has been revealed as an important source of top-down control over processing of multisensory information. While some studies demonstrated the role of long-term experience and learning (e.g., Froyen et al, 2009;Stevenson and Wallace, 2013;Barenholtz et al, 2014;Ten Oever et al, 2014;Matusz et al, 2015b), many focused on effects operating at shorter timescales, such as expectations and/or experiences built over the course of a single experimental session (e.g., Murray et al, 2004Murray et al, , 2005von Kriegstein and Giraud, 2006;Meylan and Murray, 2007;Rosenblum et al, 2007;Beierholm et al, 2009;Powers et al, 2009;Barakat et al, 2013;Chandrasekaran et al, 2009;Thelen et al, 2012Thelen et al, , 2014Matusz et al, 2015c;Altieri et al, 2015), or even across a pair of successive experimental trials Murray et al 2009;King et al 2012;Sarmiento et al, 2015). Considered together, the overwhelming evidence for the importance of context-based factors for stimulus processing across the senses and the concomitant limited existing data on the ACOP makes it plausible that irrelevant sounds activate the visual cortex in some contexts but not in others.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Given that children registered in early elementary school are not developmentally prepared to focus and sustain attention for long periods of time while staying still in their seats (Matusz et al, 2015), and that learning at this age is usually more kinesthetically-oriented (Vetter, O'Connor, O'Dwyer, & Orr, 2015) using TPR to teach FL vocabulary seems to be an age-appropriate and efficient method for elementary school FL classrooms.…”
Section: The Keyword Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%