2023
DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-30325-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Movement characteristics impact decision-making and vice versa

Abstract: Previous studies suggest that humans are capable of coregulating the speed of decisions and movements if promoted by task incentives. It is unclear however whether such behavior is inherent to the process of translating decisional information into movements, beyond posing a valid strategy in some task contexts. Therefore, in a behavioral online study we imposed time constraints to either decision- or movement phases of a sensorimotor task, ensuring that coregulating decisions and movements was not promoted by … Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

5
19
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
2

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 83 publications
(128 reference statements)
5
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Further, the occurrence of changes of mind after the onset of a movement demonstrates that the decision process overlaps with movement execution (Visser et al, 2023). Other studies have shown that speed constraints imposed on decision making can influence the speed of subsequent movements and vice versa (Carsten et al, 2023). In line with such bi-directional influences between decision-making and movement control, Kelly and O’Connell (2013) showed that a centroparietal positivity component in an EEG study scaled both with coherence of a random dot motion stimulus as well as with the reaction time of the subsequent decision.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Further, the occurrence of changes of mind after the onset of a movement demonstrates that the decision process overlaps with movement execution (Visser et al, 2023). Other studies have shown that speed constraints imposed on decision making can influence the speed of subsequent movements and vice versa (Carsten et al, 2023). In line with such bi-directional influences between decision-making and movement control, Kelly and O’Connell (2013) showed that a centroparietal positivity component in an EEG study scaled both with coherence of a random dot motion stimulus as well as with the reaction time of the subsequent decision.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The pacman was presented together with a ghost which will be described in the “Experimental Design” section, as its features depended on the experimental condition the subjects were in. Notably, the pacman animation often ended with a blank screen to cover the minimum 3000 ms period of the movement phase: as such, completing the tapping movement faster never allowed to shorten the trial duration, as in Carsten et al, 2023. Finally, note also that trials in which subjects did not provide a response before Jump -15 did not involve any pacman animation; in this case participants had instead to remain still in front of a blank screen for a period corresponding to the movement phase duration.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…That is, faster decisions are typically followed by quicker movements. Interestingly, the reverse relationship has been observed too: one study in our lab has revealed that when the context requires movements to be performed in a shorter time period, the decisions leading to them arise faster, compared to contexts in which subjects have more time to perform the same movement (Carsten et al, 2023). Altogether, these findings suggest that the urgency signal implemented in decision making models may not solely operate at this restricted level but may shape behavior in a global manner, coregulating the speed of both decisions and movements (Carsten et al, 2023; Thura, 2020).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations