1994
DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.101.1.3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon.

Abstract: The authors investigated the lexical entry for morphologically complex words in English, Six experiments, using a cross-modal repetition priming task, asked whether the lexical entry for derivationally suffixed and prefixed words is morphologically structured and how this relates to the semantic and phonological transparency of the surface relationship between stem and affix. There was clear evidence for morphological decomposition of semantically transparent forms. This was independent of phonological transpa… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

49
732
17
17

Year Published

1997
1997
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 737 publications
(831 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
49
732
17
17
Order By: Relevance
“…Positive activation is the result of activation from the input stimulus, such that the strength of activation of any lexical representation is directly proportional to its similarity to the input stimulus (Morton, 1979;McClelland and Rumelhart, 1981;Rumelhart and McClelland, 1981), as well as the result of excitatory connections among lexical forms at a lexicon-internal level, as in the case of morphologically related items (e.g., Murrell and Morton, 1974;Stanners et al, 1979;Fowler et al, 1985;Caramazza et al, 1988;Marslen-Wilson et al, 1994). Negative activation has been shown to occur under conditions in which lexical items compete for selection.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Positive activation is the result of activation from the input stimulus, such that the strength of activation of any lexical representation is directly proportional to its similarity to the input stimulus (Morton, 1979;McClelland and Rumelhart, 1981;Rumelhart and McClelland, 1981), as well as the result of excitatory connections among lexical forms at a lexicon-internal level, as in the case of morphologically related items (e.g., Murrell and Morton, 1974;Stanners et al, 1979;Fowler et al, 1985;Caramazza et al, 1988;Marslen-Wilson et al, 1994). Negative activation has been shown to occur under conditions in which lexical items compete for selection.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Negative activation has been shown to occur under conditions in which lexical items compete for selection. Such inhibition may occur among orthographically similar lexical forms (Colombo, 1986;Grainger et al, 1989;Grainger, 1990;Grainger and Segui, 1990;Segui and Grainger, 1990;Snodgrass and Mintzer, 1993; but see Grainger et al, 1992) as well as between stem homographs (Laudanna et al, 1989(Laudanna et al, , 1992 and competing suffixes (Caramazza et al, 1988;Marslen-Wilson et al, 1994).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some argue that the cross-modal priming task is preferable to the forward masked priming task because it minimizes the influence of form similarity for native speakers (Marslen-Wilson et al, 1993, Marslen-Wilson, Tyler, Waksler, & Older, 1994, as well as for non-native speakers (Feldman, Kostić & Pastizzo, under review). However, there is evidence that the cross-modal priming procedure is not fully immune to form-based effects.…”
Section: Single Processing Mechanism Account: Semantic Density and Fomentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, of those words that are not compounds or derived words, roughly a third carries an inflectional ending. The full-form approach of Shortlist B to morphological processing sets this model apart from theories assuming obligatory decomposition (see, e.g., Rastle and Davis (2008), for visual comprehension and Marslen-Wilson, Tyler, Waksler, and Older (1994), for auditory comprehension).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%