1982
DOI: 10.1086/461275
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

More on How (And How Not) to Remember the States and Their Capitals

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

1983
1983
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 5 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Let us now consider the second point, namely that strategy breakdowns occur as one moves from imagining what works to assessing what works. One particularly salient example comes from the domain of mnemonic strategy instruction, where it was found that a commercially available set of materials for remembering the states and their capitals (Lucas, 1978) did not work at all as intended when subjected to empirical test (Levin, Berry, Miller, & Bartell, 1982). The problem in that particular case turned out to be the idiosyncratic strategies generated by Lucas, as well as the complexity of those strategies when targeted for elementary school children.…”
Section: Cognitive Principle 4: Thought-to-be-effective Learning Stramentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Let us now consider the second point, namely that strategy breakdowns occur as one moves from imagining what works to assessing what works. One particularly salient example comes from the domain of mnemonic strategy instruction, where it was found that a commercially available set of materials for remembering the states and their capitals (Lucas, 1978) did not work at all as intended when subjected to empirical test (Levin, Berry, Miller, & Bartell, 1982). The problem in that particular case turned out to be the idiosyncratic strategies generated by Lucas, as well as the complexity of those strategies when targeted for elementary school children.…”
Section: Cognitive Principle 4: Thought-to-be-effective Learning Stramentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The suggestion to use multiple keywords (e.g., "frog on air" for Fragonard) may actually work against the effectiveness of a mnemonic strategy. Although the multiple-keyword issue was not addressed directly here, our experiments seem to suggest that the simpler single-keyword approach is preferable to one in which multiple keywords are "forced" to match every syllable (for experimental evidence, see Levin et al, 1982). At the same time, we recognize that in the case of artist recall, multiple keywords may be associated with a trade-off between reduced meaningfulness and enhanced phonetic correspondence that enables complete-name production.…”
Section: Effective Abstract and Interactionsmentioning
confidence: 83%
“…In addition, the compound images associated with them are often complex and nonmeaningful. In a comparison of Lucas's (1978) multiple-keyword materials for learning the capital cities of the United States with simpler single-keyword materials, Levin, Berry, Miller, and Bartell (1982) found that the latter resulted in significantly higher performance. Although not a direct test of the single-versus-multiple-keyword issue, such results suggest that the use of single keywords is preferable.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Internal mnemonic cues have long supported learning in education and do improve learning of foreign language vocabulary (Atkinson & Raugh, 1975), state capitals (J. R. Levin, Kessler Berry, Miller, & Bartell, 1982), and even information presented in prose (Shriberg, 1982). College students consistently report generating mnemonic cuesrhymes, acronyms, songs, and stories-to remember connections among important ideas (Van Etten, Freebern, & Pressley, 1997).…”
Section: Kinds Of Self-generated Cuesmentioning
confidence: 99%