2014
DOI: 10.1037/a0033598
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Moral identity as moral ideal self: Links to adolescent outcomes.

Abstract: The purposes of this study were to conceptualize moral identity as moral ideal self, to develop a measure of this construct, to test for age and gender differences, to examine links between moral ideal self and adolescent outcomes, and to assess purpose and social responsibility as mediators of the relations between moral ideal self and outcomes. Data came from a local school sample (Data Set 1: N = 510 adolescents; 10-18 years of age) and a national online sample (Data Set 2: N = 383 adolescents; 15-18 years … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

4
65
2
25

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 119 publications
(103 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
4
65
2
25
Order By: Relevance
“…A sum score is derived that reflects the self-importance of moral values (sometimes in absolute terms, sometimes relative to non-moral values). Whereas the standard model would imply a positive correlation between moral identity and adolescents' or young adults' age, this score was repeatedly found to be unrelated to age in several large-scale cross-sectional studies as well as two longitudinal studies covering the age period of mid to late adolescence [Hardy, 2006;Hardy, Walker, Olsen, Woodbury, & Hickman, 2014;Hardy, Walker, Rackham, & Olsen, 2012;Krettenauer, 2011;Pratt, Hunsberger, Pancer, & Alisat, 2003].…”
Section: The Empirical Challenge: Lack Of Evidence For Developmental mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A sum score is derived that reflects the self-importance of moral values (sometimes in absolute terms, sometimes relative to non-moral values). Whereas the standard model would imply a positive correlation between moral identity and adolescents' or young adults' age, this score was repeatedly found to be unrelated to age in several large-scale cross-sectional studies as well as two longitudinal studies covering the age period of mid to late adolescence [Hardy, 2006;Hardy, Walker, Olsen, Woodbury, & Hickman, 2014;Hardy, Walker, Rackham, & Olsen, 2012;Krettenauer, 2011;Pratt, Hunsberger, Pancer, & Alisat, 2003].…”
Section: The Empirical Challenge: Lack Of Evidence For Developmental mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Arnold, 1993;Hardy, 2006;Hardy, Walker, Olsen, Woodbury, & Hickman, 2014;Hardy, Walker, Rackham, & Olsen, 2012;Krettenauer, 2011;Pratt, Hunsberger, & Pancer, 2003). Effects of age normally do not emerge in studies with young adults, even when the well-validated measure developed by Aquino and Reed (2002) is used (Gu, 2011;Matherne & Litchfield, 2012;McFerran, Aquino, & Duffy, 2010;O'Fallon & Butterfield, 2011;Reynolds, Dang, Yam, & Leavitt, 2014).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Effects of age normally do not emerge in studies with young adults, even when the well-validated measure developed by Aquino and Reed (2002) is used (Gu, 2011;Matherne & Litchfield, 2012;McFerran, Aquino, & Duffy, 2010;O'Fallon & Butterfield, 2011;Reynolds, Dang, Yam, & Leavitt, 2014). As a consequence, researchers openly have conceded that common measures of moral identity may not be sensitive to developmental change (Hardy et al, 2014).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It opened up possibilities for engaging other psychological literatures, particularly those regarding personality and cognition, with the goal of deriving robust integrative models of moral functioning [Lapsley & Stey, 2014]. It is implicated in research on the moral ideal self [Hardy, Walker, Olsen, Woodbury, & Hickman, 2014], moral exemplars [e.g., Colby & Damon, 1992;Walker & Frimer, 2007], and social-cognitive accounts of moral personality [Aquino & Reed, 2002;Lapsley & Narvaez, 2004].…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%