1994
DOI: 10.1521/soco.1994.12.4.309
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Mood-Related Persuasion Depends on (Mis)Attributions

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

14
126
2
1

Year Published

2000
2000
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 195 publications
(143 citation statements)
references
References 2 publications
14
126
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Wegener et al, 1995). Additionally, in line with the feelings-as-information framework from which our model is derived, it is also predicted that these interactive effects of mood and message framing should be eradicated (or overcorrectively inverted) when the positive and negative affective states driving them are rendered irrelevant by the provision of source cues (Sinclair, Mark, & Clore, 1994).…”
Section: Future Directionssupporting
confidence: 53%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Wegener et al, 1995). Additionally, in line with the feelings-as-information framework from which our model is derived, it is also predicted that these interactive effects of mood and message framing should be eradicated (or overcorrectively inverted) when the positive and negative affective states driving them are rendered irrelevant by the provision of source cues (Sinclair, Mark, & Clore, 1994).…”
Section: Future Directionssupporting
confidence: 53%
“…For instance, in the domain of persuasion, it has been repeatedly demonstrated that individuals in bad moods exhibit more attitude change in response to strong, rather than weak, arguments, whereas those in good moods exhibit moderate and equal attitude change irrespective of argument quality (Bless, Bohner, Schwarz, & Strack;1990;Bless, Mackie, & Schwarz, 1992;Sinclair, Mark, & Clore, 1994). Consistent with the feelings-as-information approach, these findings suggest that negative affective states, such as bad moods, relative to positive affective states, such as good moods, increase the motivation to engage in effortful information processing, in this case leading to greater elaboration of the content of persuasive messages.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…All studies demonstrated that participants, who saw positive affective cues, were persuaded by strong and weak arguments, whereas those, who saw negative affective cues, were only affected only by strong arguments. Studies that influenced the experienced affect showed the same pattern of results (Sinclair, Mark, & Clore, 1994). Further, Friedman and Förster (2000), Study 7) demonstrated that negative affective cues facilitated analytical reasoning.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 74%
“…Compared to neutral and pleasant moods, sad moods have been found to increase the care with which people process substantive information in persuasion (Bless, Bohner, Schwarz, & Strack, 1990;Sinclair, Mark, & Clore, 1994), decrease the reliance on general knowledge structures such as scripts and stereotypes (Bless, Schwarz, Clore, Golisano, & Rabe, 1996;Bodenhausen, Kramer, & Suesser, 1994), increase the ability to estimate covariation from scatter plot data (Sinclair & Mark, 1995), reduce the susceptibility to halo effects (Sinclair, 1988), reduce fundamental attribution errors (Forgas, 1998), and increase the transitivity of preferences (Fiedler, 1988). Overall, temporary sad moods seem to trigger a more systematic, data-driven, and analytical form of reasoning.…”
Section: Effects Of Incidental Affect On Judgment and Decision Processesmentioning
confidence: 99%