1970
DOI: 10.1080/00223980.1970.9916834
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Monocular and Binocular Estimations of Distance When Knowledge of the Relevant Space is Absent

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

1976
1976
2000
2000

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 1 publication
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Though all of these shared a common methodological approach (an attempt to isolate convergence as a source of information about distance), their results were astonishingly contradictory. Some authors asserted that convergence improved distance discrimination and estimation (Wundt 1862;Baird 1903;Swenson 1932;Grant 1942;Gogel 1962;Komoda and Ono 1974;Foley 1978), whereas others argued that convergence did not serve as a cue of distance (Bappert 1923;Heinemann et al 1959;Crannell and Peters 1970). Surprisingly, there was a third group of experiments in which some observers were able to use convergence to assess a distance while others were unable to do so (Gogel 1961a(Gogel , 1961bRichards and Miller 1969).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 90%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Though all of these shared a common methodological approach (an attempt to isolate convergence as a source of information about distance), their results were astonishingly contradictory. Some authors asserted that convergence improved distance discrimination and estimation (Wundt 1862;Baird 1903;Swenson 1932;Grant 1942;Gogel 1962;Komoda and Ono 1974;Foley 1978), whereas others argued that convergence did not serve as a cue of distance (Bappert 1923;Heinemann et al 1959;Crannell and Peters 1970). Surprisingly, there was a third group of experiments in which some observers were able to use convergence to assess a distance while others were unable to do so (Gogel 1961a(Gogel , 1961bRichards and Miller 1969).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…It is interesting to note that most of the investigators who dealt with real objects as test stimuli arrived at negative conclusions concerning convergence as a cue for distance (Hillebrandt 1894;Bourdon 1902;Bappert 1923;Crannell and Peters 1970), whereas the researchers who believed that convergence played an essential role in perceiving distance derived their conclusions mainly from stereoscopic vision experiments (Swenson 1932;Grant 1942) or from experiments with the so-called 'wallpaper phenomenon' (Lie 1965;Ono et al 1971).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…As might be expected, the particular target used must constitute an effective blur stimulus for accommodation to provide changing information as a function of target distance. A very poor stimulus such as a featureless surface or a pinpoint of light, which results in little or no departure from the tonic accommodative level (Leibowitz and Owens, 1975;Owens and Leibowitz, 1976), fails to influence monocular judgements of distance (Kunnepas, 1968;Crannel and Peters, 1970) or size (Heineman, Tulving and Nachmias, 1959;Crannel and Peters, 1970) in the absence of changes in target brightness or angular extent. In contrast, a target that is highiy effective in stimulating accommodation can produce c h i s t e n t changes in monocular apparent distance under similar conditions (Fisher and Ciuffreda, 1989).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In many experiments of depth perception, however, it has been found that the accuracy in the estimation of depth deteriorated when ordinary visual cues were absent (Crannell & Peters, 1970;Epstein & Landauer, 1969;Gogel, 1972;Kiinnapas, 1968). Kiinnapas (1968) reported that when a target was placed at any distance from 25 to 395 cm in a dark room, the subjects always estimated the target distance approximately 140 cm.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%