2003
DOI: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00041-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Modulating the experience of agency: a positron emission tomography study

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

27
444
2
11

Year Published

2007
2007
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 618 publications
(484 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
27
444
2
11
Order By: Relevance
“…This incorrect gait agency during goal-directed locomotion and navigation suggests that the conscious monitoring of an agent's moving body is not reliable and that the generation of full-body locomotion and the building of a conscious experience of it are distinct brain processes. The value of 10-15°above which angular biases during cyclic locomotion were more often correctly perceived than not by our participants (no selfattribution) was compatible with earlier work on agency for goaldirected hand and arm movements (thresholds of 6.5°-15°; Farrer et al, 2003;Franck et al, 2001;Jeannerod and Pacherie, 2004;Posada et al, 2007;Slachevsky et al, 2001;Synofzik et al, 2006).…”
Section: Spatial Visuo-motor Conflicts and Full-body Agencysupporting
confidence: 91%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This incorrect gait agency during goal-directed locomotion and navigation suggests that the conscious monitoring of an agent's moving body is not reliable and that the generation of full-body locomotion and the building of a conscious experience of it are distinct brain processes. The value of 10-15°above which angular biases during cyclic locomotion were more often correctly perceived than not by our participants (no selfattribution) was compatible with earlier work on agency for goaldirected hand and arm movements (thresholds of 6.5°-15°; Farrer et al, 2003;Franck et al, 2001;Jeannerod and Pacherie, 2004;Posada et al, 2007;Slachevsky et al, 2001;Synofzik et al, 2006).…”
Section: Spatial Visuo-motor Conflicts and Full-body Agencysupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Most previous work on agency has focussed on the investigation of performance-related visual cues (Daprati et al, 1997;Farrer et al, 2003;Fourneret and Jeannerod, 1998;Franck et al, 2001;Salomon et al, 2011;Shimada et al, 2010;Tsakiris and Haggard, 2005;van den Bos and Jeannerod, 2002). Yet, the majority of our actions have consequences that are apparent across multiple sensory modalities including touch and audition.…”
Section: Temporal Auditory Motor Conflicts and Full-body Agencymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Although these tasks are only somewhat similar and could have been separated in two task distinct categories (if more studies were available), they both require the perceiver to distinguish own action perspective and goal from those of other persons. Nevertheless, these tasks consistently engage the TPJ (75% and 50%), in agreement with several similar PET studies that also reveal the TPJ as major site of brain activation Decety et al, 2002;Farrer et al, 2003;Ruby and Decety, 2001]. Action Goals: Participants are requested to identify the likely or desired end state of a story in comparison with mere physical consequences (e.g., a gulf destroying a sand castle at the beach).…”
Section: Identifying Action Intentionsupporting
confidence: 73%
“…However, BOLD signal in the anterior insula was significantly different in the comparison of non-isomorphic and isomorphic joint action. A number of experiments suggest that the insula (in addition to the TPJ) plays a role in the attribution of agency (Farrer and Frith, 2002;Farrer et al, 2003). The current finding suggests that the brain bases of attribution of agency in joint-action situations may critically depend on the exact nature of the task.…”
Section: Different Joint Action Different Functional Signaturementioning
confidence: 48%