The Handbook of Psycholinguistics 2017
DOI: 10.1002/9781118829516.ch7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Models Linking Production and Comprehension

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
16
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 136 publications
2
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In contrast, here we have shown that convergence occurs within individuals when comparing their activation profile as a speaker and as a listener. Our results can be explained by models of language processing which assume that semantic representations are shared between comprehension and production (Gambi & Pickering, 2017;Hagoort, 2013;Kintsch & van Dijk, 1978;Levelt, Roelofs, & Meyer, 1999). This shared representation may allow the production system to make forward predictions that aid comprehension (Dell & Chang, 2014;Pickering & Garrod, 2007) and help interlocutors to align their situation models during conversation .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 77%
“…In contrast, here we have shown that convergence occurs within individuals when comparing their activation profile as a speaker and as a listener. Our results can be explained by models of language processing which assume that semantic representations are shared between comprehension and production (Gambi & Pickering, 2017;Hagoort, 2013;Kintsch & van Dijk, 1978;Levelt, Roelofs, & Meyer, 1999). This shared representation may allow the production system to make forward predictions that aid comprehension (Dell & Chang, 2014;Pickering & Garrod, 2007) and help interlocutors to align their situation models during conversation .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 77%
“…Speaker-oriented and listeneroriented accounts are not mutually exclusive. Hypotheses and interpretations found in psycholinguistic literature fall on a continuum between the two views , especially if seen as a consequence of the constraints imposed by communication (Gambi and Pickering, 2017), i.e., the tension between production ease and robust message transmission (Zipf, 1935;Lindblom, 1990;Levy and Jaeger, 2007;Jaeger and Buz, 2016).…”
Section: Accounting For Probabilistic Effectsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The reafference principle has indeed been suggested as a theoretical frame for perceptual processes in general, and has been suggested to explain “pre‐emptive perception” (Bodis‐Wollner, 2008), “proactive perception” (Rimmele, Morillon, Poeppel, & Arnal, 2018), the discontinuity of thinking in schizophrenia (Feinberg, 1978), and speech production (Tian & Poeppel, 2010, 2013; Tian, Zarate, & Poeppel, 2016). Although the reafference principle has been invited into the field of language perception (Gambi & Pickering, 2017; Pickering & Clark, 2014; Pickering & Gambi, 2018; Pickering & Garrod, 2013), the reasoning is basically on the computational and algorithmic level (Marr, 1982). Neurobiological features of the principle refer mainly to perception–production connections (Dick & Andric, 2013; Pickering & Garrod, 2013) rather than to the basis of the two (or three, as we propose) operations in the reafference principle.…”
Section: A Three‐stage Cycling Model In Language Processingmentioning
confidence: 99%