2014
DOI: 10.1037/a0036837
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Modeling behavioral reactivity to losses and rewards on the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART): Moderation by alcohol problem severity.

Abstract: The relationship between risk-taking behavior and substance dependence has proven to be complex, particularly when examining across participants expressing a range of substance use problem severity. While main indices of risk-taking in the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) positively associate with problematic alcohol use in adolescent populations (e.g., MacPherson, Magidson, Reynolds, Kahler, & Lejuez, 2010), several studies have observed a negative relationship when examining behavior within adult substance … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

2
11
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
(105 reference statements)
2
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We modeled whether the number of pumps on a given trial varied depending on the outcome of the previous trial. Consistent with prior studies (Ashenhurst, Bujarski, Jentsch, & Ray, 2014; Mata, Hau, Papassotiropoulos, & Hertwig, 2012), our Level 1 equation was centerNumberofPumpsij=b0j+b1j(Explosion(N1))+b2j(Explosion(N))+b3j(TrialNumber)+εijTotal pumps on a particular trial ( i ) for a particular adolescent ( j ) was modeled as a function of the average number of pumps across the task ( b 0 j ) and whether the previous trial ( b 1 j ) was an explosion or cash-out (coded Explosion ( N − 1) = 0; Cash-Out ( N − 1) = 1). In addition, we included two controls, including whether the current trial resulted in an explosion or a cash-out ( b 2 j ; coded Explosion ( N ) = 1; Cash-Out ( N ) ) = 0) and the trial number ( b 3 j ).…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 92%
“…We modeled whether the number of pumps on a given trial varied depending on the outcome of the previous trial. Consistent with prior studies (Ashenhurst, Bujarski, Jentsch, & Ray, 2014; Mata, Hau, Papassotiropoulos, & Hertwig, 2012), our Level 1 equation was centerNumberofPumpsij=b0j+b1j(Explosion(N1))+b2j(Explosion(N))+b3j(TrialNumber)+εijTotal pumps on a particular trial ( i ) for a particular adolescent ( j ) was modeled as a function of the average number of pumps across the task ( b 0 j ) and whether the previous trial ( b 1 j ) was an explosion or cash-out (coded Explosion ( N − 1) = 0; Cash-Out ( N − 1) = 1). In addition, we included two controls, including whether the current trial resulted in an explosion or a cash-out ( b 2 j ; coded Explosion ( N ) = 1; Cash-Out ( N ) ) = 0) and the trial number ( b 3 j ).…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 92%
“…That AUD did not make riskier choices than HC is in contrast to reports of enhanced risk propensity in AUD or problem drinkers but in agreement with reports of normal choice behavior (Moallem & Ray, ) or even more risk‐averse decisions in the BART (Ashenhurst, Bujarski, Jentsch, & Ray, ; Ashenhurst, Jentsch, & Ray, ). Contrary to present expectation, impulsivity did not affect choice behavior.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 82%
“…Mata and colleagues inferred risk taking from average total number of button presses on collected trials. This study quantified behavioral modification on a trial-by-trial basis as a result of rewards and losses to assess impulsive decision-making (Ashenhurst, Bujarski, Jentsch, & Ray, 2014) and extend the findings of Mata and colleagues to show how DAT polymorphism dictates impulsive decision-making.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 62%