2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.04.016
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Modeling accuracy as a function of response time with the generalized linear mixed effects model

Abstract: a b s t r a c t a r t i c l e i n f oIn psycholinguistic studies using error rates as a response measure, response times (RT) are most often analyzed independently of the error rate, although it is widely recognized that they are related. In this paper we present a mixed effects logistic regression model for the error rate that uses RT as a trial-level fixed-and random-effect regression input. Production data from a translation-recall experiment are analyzed as an example. Several model comparisons reveal that… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
32
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8
1
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 27 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
1
32
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, this trick only means that the difficulty is merely transmogrified—now the cue integration process model is generating hypotheses about both psychological processing mechanisms and about the nature of representation. This is especially problematic because traditional dependent measures (e.g., performance on a task, brain responses, but especially reaction times) cannot discriminate between effects arising from differences in processing speed (a proxy for mechanism) and differences in representation strength or other aspect (Wickelgren, 1977 ; Davidson and Martin, 2013 ). This means that experimental designs will have to be careful not to conflate predictions about representation with predictions about mechanism itself.…”
Section: Capturing Multiple Distinctions In Parsingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, this trick only means that the difficulty is merely transmogrified—now the cue integration process model is generating hypotheses about both psychological processing mechanisms and about the nature of representation. This is especially problematic because traditional dependent measures (e.g., performance on a task, brain responses, but especially reaction times) cannot discriminate between effects arising from differences in processing speed (a proxy for mechanism) and differences in representation strength or other aspect (Wickelgren, 1977 ; Davidson and Martin, 2013 ). This means that experimental designs will have to be careful not to conflate predictions about representation with predictions about mechanism itself.…”
Section: Capturing Multiple Distinctions In Parsingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The efficiency score shares the property with other recent suggestions for accounting for SATOs (Davidson & Martin, 2013;Seli, Jonker, Cheyne, & Smilek, 2013) that it assumes a linear relation between response time and accuracy. While such approaches may be better than focusing on a single behavioral variable, the assumption of linearity is at odds with work which has explicitly characterized the SATO (Fitts, 1966;Heitz, 2014;Wickelgren, 1977) and has shown a distinctly curvilinear relation between response time and accuracy.…”
Section: Speed-accuracy Trade-offsmentioning
confidence: 60%
“…Response Time Response time in this study is the time it takes to respond to each nonword. In many behavioural tasks response time is included as a control for the wellknown trade-off between speed and accuracy (e.g., Heitz, 2014;Davidson and Martin, 2013). This is not how we should interpret response time because there is not a correct or an incorrect answer to whether a given nonword belongs to the artificial language exposed to the participants.…”
Section: Trial Numbermentioning
confidence: 99%