2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.joep.2016.03.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Missing the best opportunity; who can seize the next one? Agents show less inaction inertia than personal decision makers

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
12
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
0
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Our sample size was based on a goal of at least 40 participants in each condition, based on past experimental experiences and other studies (e.g., Lu, Jia, et al, 2016; Wang et al, 2018). Accordingly, 87 university students (34 males, M age = 22.90 years, SD age = 3.34) were recruited through the Bulletin Board System (BBS) of Peking University for monetary payment (¥10).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Our sample size was based on a goal of at least 40 participants in each condition, based on past experimental experiences and other studies (e.g., Lu, Jia, et al, 2016; Wang et al, 2018). Accordingly, 87 university students (34 males, M age = 22.90 years, SD age = 3.34) were recruited through the Bulletin Board System (BBS) of Peking University for monetary payment (¥10).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Individuals make financial decisions for themselves and for others. A growing body of literature on self–other discrepancies has suggested that decision makers (i.e., people who make decisions) perform differently when they decide for others rather than for themselves (e.g., Y. Liu, Polman, Liu, & Jiao, 2018; Z. Liu, Liu, & Mu, 2017; Lu, Jia, Xie, & Wang, 2016; Lu & Xie, 2014; Lu, Xie, & Xu, 2012; Polman, 2012; Polman & Vohs, 2016; Tunney & Ziegler, 2015). A decision target (i.e., whether the decision is made for oneself or others) influences decision makers' behaviors (Lu et al, 2012), especially on risk‐related decisions (e.g., Andersson, Holm, Tyran, & Wengström, 2016; Polman & Wu, 2020; Stone & Allgaier, 2008; Sun, Liu, Zhang, & Lu, 2016).…”
Section: Self–other Decision Differencesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The inaction inertia principle suggests that after having missed an attractive opportunity, the likelihood of accepting a subsequent less attractive opportunity in the same domain decreases (Tykocinski et al, ). When people miss an attractive opportunity, they regard a later less attractive opportunity as a loss relative to the missed superior opportunity (Lu et al, ). As consumers wish to avoid feeling like a “sucker” when they buy a product that now costs more than it did a short time ago (i.e., anticipation regret), and they want to escape the negative emotions associated with failing to take advantage of a bargain (i.e., experienced regret; Arkes et al, ; Butler & Highhouse, ; Sevdalis, Harvey, & Yip, ; Tykocinski et al, ), they often reject the current opportunity.…”
Section: Literature Review and Hypotheses Developmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, inaction inertia can decrease consumer acceptance of a less attractive product or promotion, threatening practitioners’ profits. Therefore, a better understanding of why the inaction inertia is elicited and how it can be lowered has been an attractive research subject, which is evident by the many studies that have examined this phenomenon (e.g., Arkes, Kung, & Hutzel, ; Kumar, ; Liu, Cheng, & Ni, ; Lu, Jia, Xie, & Wang, ; Terris & Tykocinski, ; Tykocinski & Pittman, ; Tykocinski et al, ; Zeelenberg & Van Putten, ). However, previous studies have mostly focused on inaction inertia invoked after consumers have missed opportunities to buy single products or services.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%